HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Subject–verb inversion in English is a type of inversion marked by a
predicate Predicate or predication may refer to: * Predicate (grammar), in linguistics * Predication (philosophy) * several closely related uses in mathematics and formal logic: **Predicate (mathematical logic) **Propositional function **Finitary relation, o ...
verb A verb is a word that generally conveys an action (''bring'', ''read'', ''walk'', ''run'', ''learn''), an occurrence (''happen'', ''become''), or a state of being (''be'', ''exist'', ''stand''). In the usual description of English, the basic f ...
that precedes a corresponding subject, e.g., "Beside the bed ''stood a lamp''". Subject–verb inversion is distinct from
subject–auxiliary inversion Subject–auxiliary inversion (SAI; also called subject–operator inversion) is a frequently occurring type of inversion (linguistics), inversion in the English language whereby a finite auxiliary verb – taken here to include finite forms of th ...
because the verb involved is not an
auxiliary verb An auxiliary verb ( abbreviated ) is a verb that adds functional or grammatical meaning to the clause in which it occurs, so as to express tense, aspect, modality, voice, emphasis, etc. Auxiliary verbs usually accompany an infinitive verb or ...
.


Overview

The following sentences illustrate subject–verb inversion. They compare canonical order with the more marked inversion order, and they demonstrate that subject–verb inversion is unlikely if the subject is a weak (non-stressed) definite pronoun: ::a. Jim sat under the tree. ::b. Under the tree sat Jim. - Subject–verb inversion ::c. *Under the tree sat he. - Subject–verb inversion unlikely with weak definite subject pronoun ::a. The dog came down the stairs. ::b. Down the stairs came the dog. - Subject–verb inversion ::c. *Down the stairs came it. - Subject–verb inversion unlikely with weak definite subject pronoun ::a. Some flowers are in the vase. ::b. In the vase are some flowers. - Subject–verb inversion with the copula ::c. *In the vase are they. - Subject–verb inversion unlikely with weak definite subject pronoun ::a. Bill said, "I am hungry." ::b. "I am hungry," said Bill. - Subject–verb–object inversion ::c. "I am hungry," said he. - Subject–verb–object inversion here possible, but less likely, with weak definite subject pronoun Subject–verb inversion has occurred in the b-sentences to emphasize the post-verb subject. The emphasis may occur, for instance, to establish a contrast of the subject with another entity in the discourse context.


Types of subject–verb inversion

A number of types of subject–verb inversion can be acknowledged based upon the nature of phrase that precede the verb and the nature of the verb(s) involved. The following subsections enumerate four distinct types of subject–verb inversion: locative inversion, directive inversion, copular inversion, and quotative inversion.


Locative inversion

Locative inversion also occurs in many languages, including
Brazilian Portuguese Brazilian Portuguese (; ; also known as pt-BR) is the set of Variety (linguistics), varieties of Portuguese language native to Brazil. It is spoken by almost all of the 203 million inhabitants of Brazil and widely across the Brazilian diaspora ...
,
Mandarin Chinese Mandarin ( ; zh, s=, t=, p=Guānhuà, l=Mandarin (bureaucrat), officials' speech) is the largest branch of the Sinitic languages. Mandarin varieties are spoken by 70 percent of all Chinese speakers over a large geographical area that stretch ...
,
Otjiherero Herero (Otjiherero) is a Bantu language spoken by the Herero and Mbanderu peoples in Namibia and Botswana, as well as by small communities of people in southwestern Angola. There were speakers in these countries between 2015 and 2018. Distr ...
, Chichewa, and a number of Germanic and
Bantu languages The Bantu languages (English: , Proto-Bantu language, Proto-Bantu: *bantʊ̀), or Ntu languages are a language family of about 600 languages of Central Africa, Central, Southern Africa, Southern, East Africa, Eastern and Southeast Africa, South ...
. A predicative phrase is switched from its default postverbal position to a position preceding the verb, which causes the subject and the finite verb to invert. For example: ::a. A lamp lay in the corner. ::b. In the corner lay a lamp. – Locative inversion ::c. *In the corner lay it. – Locative inversion unlikely with a weak pronoun subject ::a. Only Larry sleeps under that tree. ::b. Under that tree sleeps only Larry. – Locative inversion ::c. *Under that tree sleeps he. – Locative inversion unlikely with a weak pronoun subject The fronted expression that evokes locative inversion is an adjunct of location. Locative inversion in modern English is a vestige of the V2 order associated with earlier stages of the language.


Directive inversion

Directive inversion is closely related to locative inversion insofar as the pre-verb expression denotes a location, the only difference being that the verb is now a verb of movement. Typical verbs that allow directive inversion in English are ''come'', ''go'', ''run'', etc. ::a. Two students came into the room. ::b. Into the room came two students. – Directive inversion ::c. *Into the room came they. – Directive inversion unlikely with a weak pronoun subject ::a. The squirrel fell out of the tree. ::b. Out of the tree fell the squirrel. – Directive inversion ::c. *Out of the tree fell it. – Directive inversion unlikely with a weak pronoun subject The fronted expression that evokes inversion is a directive expression; it helps express movement toward a destination. The following sentence may also be an instance of directive inversion, although the fronted expression expresses time rather than direction: ::a. The toasts came after the speeches. ::b. After the speeches came the toasts. – Inversion after a time expression Like locative inversion, directive inversion is undoubtedly a vestige of the V2 word order associated with earlier stages of the language.


Copular inversion

Copular inversion occurs when a predicative nominal switches positions with the subject in a clause where the copula ''be'' is the
finite verb A finite verb is a verb that contextually complements a subject, which can be either explicit (like in the English indicative) or implicit (like in null subject languages or the English imperative). A finite transitive verb or a finite intra ...
. The result of this inversion is known as an inverse copular construction, e.g. ::a. Bill is our representative. ::b. Our representative is Bill. – Copular inversion ::c. *Our representative is he. – Copular inversion unlikely with weak pronoun subject ::a. The objection was a concern. ::b. A concern was the objection. – Copular inversion ::c. *A concern was it. – Copular inversion unlikely with weak pronoun subject This type of inversion occurs with a finite form of the copula ''be''. Since English predominantly has SV order, it will tend to view whichever noun phrase immediately precedes the finite verb as the subject. Thus in the second b-sentence, ''A concern'' is taken as the subject, and ''the objection'' as the predicate. But if one acknowledges that copular inversion has occurred, one can argue that ''the objection '' is the subject; and ''A concern,'' the predicate. This confusion has led to focused study of these types of copular clauses. Where there is a difference in
number A number is a mathematical object used to count, measure, and label. The most basic examples are the natural numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, and so forth. Numbers can be represented in language with number words. More universally, individual numbers can ...
, the verb agrees with the noun phrase that precedes it: ::a. Jack and Jill are the problem. ::b. The problem is Jack and Jill. – On an inversion analysis, the verb agrees with the apparent predicate.


Quotative inversion

In literature, subject–verb inversion occurs with verbs that attribute speech to a character. The inversion follows an instance of
direct speech Direct may refer to: Mathematics * Directed set, in order theory * Direct limit of (pre), sheaves * Direct sum of modules, a construction in abstract algebra which combines several vector spaces Computing * Direct access (disambiguation), a ...
that typically occurs in quotation marks: ::a. "We are going to win," Bill said. ::b. "We are going to win," said Bill. – Quotative inversion ::c. *"We are going to win," said he. – Quotative inversion less likely with weak subject pronoun ::a. "What was the problem?" Larry asked. ::b. "What was the problem?" asked Larry. – Quotative inversion ::c. *"What was the problem?" asked he. – Quotative inversion less likely with weak subject pronoun This sort of inversion is almost entirely absent from everyday speech. It occurs almost exclusively in literary contexts.


Multiple verbs

Subject–verb inversion can sometimes involve more than one verb. In these cases, the subject follows all of the verbs, the finite as well as non-finite ones, e.g. ::a. Remnants of marijuana consumption have been found under her bed twice. ::b. Under her bed have been found remnants of marijuana consumption twice. ::c. Under her bed have been found twice remnants of marijuana consumption. Sentence b and sentence c, where the subject follows all the verbs, stand in stark contrast to what occurs in cases of subject–auxiliary inversion, which have the subject appearing between the finite auxiliary verb and the non-finite verb(s), e.g. ::d. Has anything been found under her bed? Further, the flexibility across sentence b and sentence c demonstrates that there is some freedom of word order in the post-verb domain. This freedom is consistent with an analysis in terms of rightwards shifting of the subject, where heavier constituents tend to follow lighter ones. Evidence for this claim comes from the observation that equivalents of sentence c above are not as good with a light subject: ::e. ?? Under her bed has been found twice marijuana. ::f. * Under her bed has been found twice it. These facts clearly distinguish this kind of inversion from simple subject–auxiliary inversion, which applies regardless of the weight of the subject: ::g. Has it been found under her bed? Thus, it is not clear from these examples if subject–auxiliary inversion is a unified grammatical phenomenon with the other cases discussed above.


Structural analysis

Like most types of inversion, subject–verb inversion is a phenomenon that challenges theories of sentence structure. In particular, the traditional subject
predicate Predicate or predication may refer to: * Predicate (grammar), in linguistics * Predication (philosophy) * several closely related uses in mathematics and formal logic: **Predicate (mathematical logic) **Propositional function **Finitary relation, o ...
division of the clause (S → NP VP) is difficult to maintain in light of instances of subject–verb inversion such as ''Into the room will come a unicorn''. Such sentences are more consistent with a theory that takes sentence structure to be relatively flat, lacking a finite
verb phrase In linguistics, a verb phrase (VP) is a syntax, syntactic unit composed of a verb and its argument (linguistics), arguments except the subject (grammar), subject of an independent clause or coordinate clause. Thus, in the sentence ''A fat man quic ...
constituent, i.e. lacking the VP of S → NP VP. In order to maintain the traditional subject–predicate division, one has to assume movement (or copying) on a massive scale. The basic difficulty is suggested by the following trees representing the phrase structures of the sentences: The convention is used here where the words themselves appear as the labels on the nodes in the trees. The tree on the left shows the canonical analysis of the clause, whereby the sentence is divided into two immediate constituents, the subject ''Bill'' and the finite VP ''crouched in the bush''. To maintain the integrity of the finite VP constituent ''crouched in the bush'', one can assume a rearranging of the constituents in the second sentence on the right, whereby both ''crouched'' and ''in the bush'' move out of the VP and up the structure. The account suggested with the second tree is the sort of analysis that one is likely to find in
Government and Binding Theory Government and binding (GB, GBT) is a theory of syntax and a phrase structure grammar in the tradition of transformational grammar developed principally by Noam Chomsky in the 1980s. This theory is a radical revision of his earlier theories and was ...
or the
Minimalist Program In linguistics, the minimalist program is a major line of inquiry that has been developing inside generative grammar since the early 1990s, starting with a 1993 paper by Noam Chomsky. Following Imre Lakatos's distinction, Chomsky presents minima ...
. It is a phrase structure account that relies on unseen movement/copying mechanisms below the surface. The unseen mechanisms must perform an even greater job for the marijuana-example above. That sentence (sentence c in the previous section) would necessitate at least five instances of movement/copying in order to maintain the presence of an underlying finite VP constituent. This makes it unlikely that the mechanism discussed above is the correct analysis for the marijuana-examples, as these might be generated by the same mechanisms that underlie
extraposition Extraposition is a mechanism of syntax that alters word order in such a manner that a relatively "heavy" constituent appears to the right of its canonical position. Extraposing a constituent results in a discontinuity and in this regard, it i ...
and heavy-NP shift. An alternative analysis of subject–verb inversion rejects the existence of the finite VP constituent. Due to the absence of this constituent, the structure is flatter, which simplifies matters considerably. The sentences with inverted order will often not result in a discontinuity, which means the basic hierarchy of constituents (the vertical order) does not change across the canonical and inverted variants. The following trees illustrate this alternative account. The first two trees illustrate the analysis in an unorthodox phrase structure grammar that rejects the presence of the finite VP constituent, and the second two trees illustrate the analysis in a dependency grammar. Dependency grammar rejects the presence of a finite VP constituent.Concerning the dependency grammar rejection of a finite VP constituent, see Tesnière (1959:103–105), Matthews (2007:17ff.), Miller (2011:54ff.), and Osborne et al. (2011:323f.). Because there is no finite VP constituent in these trees, the basic hierarchy of constituents remains consistent. What changes is just the linear order of the constituents. The following trees illustrates the "flat" dependency-based analysis of the marijuana-example. Due to the lack of a finite VP constituent, the basic hierarchy of constituents is not altered by inversion. However, this analysis does not capture the obvious dependency between the main verb and the inverted subject.


See also

*
English subjunctive While the English language lacks distinct inflections for mood, an English subjunctive is recognized in most grammars. Definition and scope of the concept vary widely across the literature, but it is generally associated with the description of ...


Notes


Literature

*Culicover, P. 1997. Principles and parameters: An introduction to syntactic theory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. *Downing, A. and Locke, P. 1992. English grammar: A university course, second edition. London: Routledge. *Greenbaum, S. and R. Quirk. 1990. A student's grammar of the English language. Harlow, Essex, England: Longman. *Groß, T. and T. Osborne 2009. Toward a practical dependency grammar theory of discontinuities. SKY Journal of Linguistics 22, 43-90. *Matthews, P. H. (2007). Syntactic Relations: a critical survey (1. publ. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. . Retrieved 24 August 2012. *Mikkelsen, Line 2005. Copular clauses: Specification, predication, and equation. Linguistics Today 85. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. *Miller, J. 2011
A critical introduction to syntax
London: continuum. *Moro, A. 1997. The raising of predicates: Predicative noun phrases and the theory of clause structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. *Osborne, T., M. Putnam, and T. Groß 2011. Bare phrase structure, label-less trees, and specifier-less syntax: Is Minimalism becoming a dependency grammar? The Linguistic Review 28, 315–364. *Quirk, R. S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, and J. Svartvik. 1979. A grammar of contemporary English. London: Longman. *Tesnière, L. 1959. Éleménts de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck. *Tesnière, L. 1969. Éleménts de syntaxe structurale, 2nd edition. Paris: Klincksieck. {{DEFAULTSORT:Subject-verb inversion in English English grammar Word order Syntactic entities Syntactic transformation