Ruki Sound Law
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

The ruki sound law, also known as the ruki rule or iurk rule, is a historical sound change that took place in the satem branches of the
Indo-European The Indo-European languages are a language family native to the northern Indian subcontinent, most of Europe, and the Iranian plateau with additional native branches found in regions such as Sri Lanka, the Maldives, parts of Central Asia (e. ...
language family, namely in Balto-Slavic, Armenian, and Indo-Iranian. According to this sound law, an original changed to (a sound similar to English ⟨sh⟩) after the consonants , , , and the semi-vowels (*u̯) and (*i̯), as well as the syllabic allophones , , and : : > / _ Specifically, the initial stage involves the retraction of the coronal
sibilant Sibilants (from 'hissing') are fricative and affricate consonants of higher amplitude and pitch, made by directing a stream of air with the tongue towards the teeth. Examples of sibilants are the consonants at the beginning of the English w ...
after semi-vowels, , or a velar consonant , or . In the second stage, leveling of the sibilant system resulted in retroflexion (cf.
Sanskrit Sanskrit (; stem form ; nominal singular , ,) is a classical language belonging to the Indo-Aryan languages, Indo-Aryan branch of the Indo-European languages. It arose in northwest South Asia after its predecessor languages had Trans-cultural ...
ष and Proto-Slavic), and later retraction to velar in Slavic and some Middle Indic languages. This rule was first formulated by Holger Pedersen, and it is sometimes known as ''Pedersen's law'', although this term is also applied to another sound law concerning stress in the Balto-Slavic languages. The name "ruki" comes from the sounds (r, u, K, i) which triggered the sound change. The law is stated as a mnemonic rule because the word руки means 'hands, arms' in Russian, or is the genitive singular form ('of the hand') of рука (compare ''rule of thumb'').


Applications to language groups

The rule was originally formulated for
Sanskrit Sanskrit (; stem form ; nominal singular , ,) is a classical language belonging to the Indo-Aryan languages, Indo-Aryan branch of the Indo-European languages. It arose in northwest South Asia after its predecessor languages had Trans-cultural ...
. It was later proposed to be valid in some degree for all satem languages, and exceptionless for the Indo-Iranian languages. (There appears to be one exception at least in some Nuristani languages, however.) In Baltic and Albanian, it is limited or affected to a greater or lesser extent by other sound laws. Nevertheless, it has to have been universal in these branches of the IE languages, and the lack of Slavic reflexes before consonants is due rather to their merger with the reflexes of other sibilants.


Exceptions in Slavic languages

In
Slavic languages The Slavic languages, also known as the Slavonic languages, are Indo-European languages spoken primarily by the Slavs, Slavic peoples and their descendants. They are thought to descend from a proto-language called Proto-Slavic language, Proto- ...
the process is regular before a vowel, but it does not take place before consonants. The final result is the voiceless velar fricative , which is even more retracted than the . This velar fricative changed back into before a
front vowel A front vowel is a class of vowel sounds used in some spoken languages, its defining characteristic being that the highest point of the tongue is positioned approximately as far forward as possible in the mouth without creating a constriction th ...
or the
palatal approximant The voiced palatal approximant is a type of consonant used in many spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is ; the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is j, and in the Americanist phonetic notation i ...
.


Exceptions in Indo-Iranian languages

In Indo-Iranian *''r'' and *''l'' merged, and the change worked even after the new sound; e.g. Avestan ''karš-'', Sanskrit ''kárṣati'' 'to plough' < PIE . This has been cited as evidence by many scholars as an argument for the later influence of
Iranian languages The Iranian languages, also called the Iranic languages, are a branch of the Indo-Iranian languages in the Indo-European language family that are spoken natively by the Iranian peoples, predominantly in the Iranian Plateau. The Iranian langu ...
on Proto-Slavic. There are obvious drawbacks in the theory. First, the two sounds must have been very close (''r''/''l''), so that both could have triggered the change in Indo-Iranian. Second, there are no real examples of this change working in Slavic, and it is also doubtful that only this change (ruki) and no other such change of sibilants (e.g. > ''h'') was borrowed into Slavic. The syllabic laryngeal *H̥ becomes *i in Proto-Indo-Iranian, and this also triggered RUKI. A later extension of RUKI was particular to the Iranian languages: *s, *z shift to *š, *ž also after the labial stops *p, *b, including even secondary *s from Proto-Indo-Iranian *ć < PIE *ḱ.


Nuristani

The Ruki rule also displays a rather different behavior in Nuristani, conditioned by the following factors: * The reflex of the Proto-Indo-European sequence *ḱs is Nuristani *ć (pronounced ͡s, the same as that of plain *ḱ, thus in this context there is no evidence of the operation of RUKI. For example, the word for "bear", reflecting Proto-Indo-European *h₂ŕ̥tḱos (Sanskrit ''ṛ́kṣa'' "bear", Avestan ''arṣ̌a'') shows a dental affricate in most Nuristani languages, such as Ashkun and Katë ''ić'', and Nuristani Kalasha ''oć''. * Proto-Indo-European sequences *ks and *kʷs appears to have become Nuristani *c̣. Thus Proto-Indo-European *ksurós "razor" is reflected as ''kṣurá'' in Sanskrit, but ''c̣uři'' "sickle" in the Southeastern Katë dialect. * Various cases where RUKI failed to operate after *i and *u in Nuristani exist. Hegedűs notes that these all seem to trace back to PIE etyma where the *us and *is sequences were earlier *uHs and *iHs, meaning the laryngeals seem to have blocked the operation of RUKI. For example, PIE *múh₂s "mouse" > Sanskrit ''mū́ṣ'', Avestan ''mūš'', but Southeastern Katë ''musë'', Prasun ''mǖsu'', while Nuristani Kalasha ''pusa'' is of dubious etymology on account of the different initial consonant, and Ashkun ''moṣ'' shows the Indo-Aryan reflex, probably due to influence or borrowing from Indo-Aryan. * Proto-Indo-European *rs and *ls merge into Nuristani *ṣ, thus after *r we do actually see proper RUKI-like behavior in Nuristani, such as Proto-Indo-European *kʷels- yielding Southeastern Katë ''kṣō-'' "to pull".


Albanian

According to Orel (2000: 62), Albanian shows a limited RUKI-like development, where ''*s'' becomes ''sh'' only after PIE *y, *w (including their vocalic counterparts ''*i, *u''). E.g. * ''lesh'' 'wool, fleece, hair' < *laysa, cf. Slavic *listъ 'leaf' < *leys-to- * ''dash'' 'ram' < *dawsa, cf. Germanic *deuzą < *dʰews-om * ''pishë'' 'pine' < *pisā, cf. Latin ''pīnus'' 'pine' < *pi(t)snos * ''prush'' 'ember' < *prusa, cf. Latin ''prūna'' 'ember' < *prusnā; Sanskrit ''ploṣati'' 'to burn' < *prews- This differs from the development of ''*rs'', ''*ks'', and of ''*s'' after other vowels, e.g. * ''djerr'' 'fallow land' < *dersa, cf. Greek ''χέρσος'' 'dry land' < *ǵʰers- * ''hirrë'' 'whey' < *ksirā, cf. Sanskrit क्षीर / ''kṣīrá'' 'milk' * ''kohë'' 'time' < *kāsā, cf. Slavic *časъ 'time' < *kʷeh₁s-eh₂ However, this view of Albanian is controversial. Firstly, the words in question that Orel bases this theory on have shaky etymologies. ''Dash'' has a disputed etymology, with rival versions attributing the word not to Proto-Indo-European *dʰews-om but instead *dʰeh₁-l-, or *demh₂ from *dmh₂ "to tame". ''Pishë'' meanwhile is argued to not be inherited from Proto-Indo-European at all; rather it and its soundalikes in Greek and Latin are in fact substrate vocabulary. ''Lesh'' is alternatively attributed instead to *h₂welh₁- "wool", making it cognate to Latin ''vellus''. Meanwhile, no RUKI-like rule is included in other studies of Proto-Albanian diachrony. Michiel de Vaan (2015) instead has a Proto-Albanian *ʃ emerging from different means, which barely resemble a RUKI law: Indo-European *ks shares the fate of simple *s in becoming *ʃ before *t (as occurred for ''jashtë'' "outside" and ''gjashtë'' "six", but not other cases with *ks where *t did not follow), with *t as the conditioning factor, rather than the prior *k. Meanwhile, the development of *s itself is highly disputed, but in contrast to Orel's view that it was conditioned on a RUKI-like phenomenon, De Vaan prefers Kortlandt'sKortlandt, Frederik (1987). Reflexes of Indo-European consonants in Albanian. ''Orpheus 8'' (Georgiev Memorial Volume): 35−37. view that *s became *ʃ when either followed by an unstressed vowel or intervocalically, regardless of the quality of nearby vowels.


Notes


Further reading

* Charles Prescott.
Germanic and the Ruki Dialects
{{Proto-Indo-European language Indo-European sound laws Slavic phonological features