HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Language production is the production of spoken or written language. In
psycholinguistics Psycholinguistics or psychology of language is the study of the interrelation between linguistic factors and psychological aspects. The discipline is mainly concerned with the mechanisms by which language is processed and represented in the mind ...
, it describes all of the stages between having a concept to express and translating that concept into linguistic forms. These stages have been described in two types of processing models: the lexical access models and the serial models. Through these models, psycholinguists can look into how speeches are produced in different ways, such as when the speaker is
bilingual Multilingualism is the use of more than one language, either by an individual speaker or by a group of speakers. When the languages are just two, it is usually called bilingualism. It is believed that multilingual speakers outnumber monolin ...
. Psycholinguists learn more about these models and different kinds of speech by using language production research methods that include collecting speech errors and elicited production tasks.


Stages involved

Language production consists of several interdependent processes which transform a nonlinguistic message into a spoken, signed, or written linguistic signal. Though the following steps proceed in this approximate order, there are plenty of interaction and communication between them. The process of message planning is an active area of psycholinguistic research, but researchers have found that it is an ongoing process throughout language production. Research suggests that messages are planned in roughly the same order that they are in an
utterance In spoken language analysis, an utterance is a continuous piece of speech, by one person, before or after which there is silence on the part of the person. In the case of oral language, spoken languages, it is generally, but not always, bounded ...
. But, there is also evidence that suggests the verbs that give case may be planned earlier than objects, even when the object is said first. After identifying a message, or part of a message, to be linguistically encoded, a speaker must select the individual words—also known as
lexical item In lexicography, a lexical item is a single word, a part of a word, or a chain of words (catena (linguistics), catena) that forms the basic elements of a language's lexicon (≈ vocabulary). Examples are ''cat'', ''traffic light'', ''take ca ...
s—to represent that message. This process is called lexical selection. The words are selected based on their meaning, which in linguistics is called
semantic Semantics is the study of linguistic Meaning (philosophy), meaning. It examines what meaning is, how words get their meaning, and how the meaning of a complex expression depends on its parts. Part of this process involves the distinction betwee ...
information. Lexical selection activates the word's lemma, which contains both semantic and grammatical information about the word. This grammatical information is then used in the next step of language production, grammatical encoding. Critical grammatical information includes characteristics such as the word's
syntactic category A syntactic category is a syntactic unit that theories of syntax assume. Word classes, largely corresponding to traditional parts of speech (e.g. noun, verb, preposition, etc.), are syntactic categories. In phrase structure grammars, the ''phrasa ...
(noun, verb, etc.), what objects it takes, and
grammatical gender In linguistics, a grammatical gender system is a specific form of a noun class system, where nouns are assigned to gender categories that are often not related to the real-world qualities of the entities denoted by those nouns. In languages wit ...
if it is present in the language. Using some of these characteristics as well as information about the
thematic roles Thematic role is a linguistic notion, which may refer to: * Theta role (in syntax or at the syntax-semantics interface), the formal device for representing syntactic argument structure—the number and type of noun phrases—required syntacticall ...
of each word in the intended message, each word is then assigned the grammatical and thematic role it will have in the sentence. Function morphemes, like the plural /s/ or the past tense /ɪd/, are added in this stage as well. After an utterance, or part of one, has been formed, it then goes through phonological encoding. In this stage of language production, the
mental representation A mental representation (or cognitive representation), in philosophy of mind, cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and cognitive science, is a hypothetical internal cognitive symbol that represents external reality or its abstractions. Mental re ...
of the words to be spoken is transformed into a sequence of speech sounds to be pronounced. The speech sounds are assembled in the order they are to be produced. The basic loop occurring in the creation of language consists of the following stages: * Intended message * Encode message into linguistic form * Encode linguistic form into speech
motor system The motor system is the set of central nervous system, central and peripheral nervous system, peripheral structures in the nervous system that support motor functions, i.e. movement. Peripheral structures may include skeletal muscles and Efferen ...
* Sound goes from speaker's mouth to hearer's ear
auditory system The auditory system is the sensory system for the sense of hearing. It includes both the ear, sensory organs (the ears) and the auditory parts of the sensory system. System overview The outer ear funnels sound vibrations to the eardrum, incre ...
* Speech is decoded into linguistic form * Linguistic form is decoded into meaning According to the lexical access model (see section below), in terms of lexical access, two different stages of cognition are employed; thus, this concept is known as the two-stage theory of lexical access. The first stage, lexical selection provides information about lexical items required to construct the functional level representation. These items are retrieved according to their specific semantic and syntactic properties, but phonological forms are not yet made available at this stage. The second stage, retrieval of wordforms, provides information required for building the positional level representation.


Models


Serial model

A serial model of language production divides the process into several stages. For example, there may be one stage for determining pronunciation and a stage for determining lexical content. The serial model does not allow overlap of these stages, so they may only be completed one at a time.


Connectionist model

Several researchers have proposed a connectionist model, one notable example being Dell. According to his connectionist model, there are four layers of processing and understanding: semantic, syntactic, morphological, and phonological. These work in parallel and in series, with activation at each level. Interference and misactivation can occur at any of these stages. Production begins with concepts, and continues down from there. One might start with the concept of a cat: a four-legged, furry, domesticated mammal with whiskers, etc. This conceptual set would attempt to find the corresponding word . This selected word would then select morphological and phonological data /k / at/. The distinction of this model is that, during this process, other elements would also be primed ( might be somewhat primed, for example), as they are physically similar, and so can cause conceptual interference. Errors might also occur at the phoneme level, as many words are phonetically similar, e.g. mat. Substitutions of similar consonant sounds are more likely to occur, e.g. between plosive stop consonants such as d, p and b. Lower primed words are less likely to be chosen, but interference is thought to occur in cases of early selection, where the level of activation of the target and interference words is at the same level.


Lexical access model

This model states that the sentence is made by a sequence of processes generating differing levels of representations. For instance, the functional level representation is made on the preverbal representation, which is essentially what the speaker seeks to express. This level is responsible for encoding the meanings of lexical items and the way that grammar forms relationships between them. Next, the positional level representation is built, which functions to encode the phonological forms of words and the order they are found in sentence structures. Lexical access, according to this model, is a process that encompasses two serially ordered and independent stages.


Additional aspects


Fluency

Fluency can be defined in part by prosody, which is shown graphically by a smooth intonation contour, and by a number of other elements: control of speech rate, relative timing of stressed and unstressed syllables, changes in amplitude, changes in fundamental frequency. In other words, fluency can be described as whether someone speaks smoothly and easily. This term is used in speech-language pathology when describing disorders with
stuttering Stuttering, also known as stammering, is a speech disorder characterized externally by involuntary repetitions and prolongations of sounds, syllables, words, or phrases as well as involuntary silent pauses called blocks in which the person who ...
or other disfluencies.


Multilingualism

Whether or not a speaker is fluent in one or more languages, the process for producing language remains the same. However, bilinguals speaking two languages within a conversation may have access to both languages at the same time. Three of the most commonly discussed models for multilingual language access are the Bilingual Interactive Activation Plus model, the Revised Hierarchical Model, and the Language Mode model: * Bilingual Interactive Activation Plus, updated from a model made by Dijkstra and Van Heuven, uses solely bottom-up processing to facilitate bilingual language access. This model suggests that the lexicon for bilingual speakers combines the languages, and access occurs across both languages at the same time. * Revised Hierarchical Model, developed by Kroll and Stewart, is a model suggesting that bilingual brains store meanings in a common place, word-forms are separated by language. * Language Mode Model, made by Grosjean, uses two assumptions to map bilingual language production in a modular way. These assumptions are that a base language is activated in conversation, and that the speaker's other language is activated to relative degrees depending on context. De Bot describes it as overly simple for the complexity of the process and suggests it has room for expansion. Speakers fluent in multiple languages may inhibit access to one of their languages, but this suppression can only be done once the speaker is at a certain level of proficiency in that language. A speaker can decide to inhibit a language based on non-linguistic cues in their conversation, such as a speaker of both English and French inhibiting their French when conversing with people who only speak English. When especially proficient multilingual speakers communicate, they can participate in
code-switching In linguistics, code-switching or language alternation occurs when a speaker alternates between two or more languages, or language varieties, in the context of a single conversation or situation. These alternations are generally intended to ...
. Code-switching has been shown to indicate bilingual proficiency in a speaker, though it had previously been seen as a sign of poor language ability.


Research methods

There are three main types of research into language production: speech error collection, picture-naming, and elicited production. Speech error collection focuses on using the analysis of speech errors made in naturally produced speech. On the other hand, elicited production focuses on elicited speech and is conducted in a lab. Also conducted in a lab, picture-naming focuses on reaction-time data from picture-naming latencies. Although originally disparate, these three methodologies are generally looking at the same underlying processes of speech production.


Speech errors

Speech errors have been found to be common in naturally produced speech. Analysis of speech errors has found that not all are random, but rather systematic and fall into several categories. These speech errors can demonstrate parts of the language processing system, and what happens when that system doesn't work as it should. Language production occurs quickly with speakers saying a little more than 2 words per second; so though errors occur only once out of 1,000 words, they occur relatively often throughout a speaker's day at once every 7 minutes. Some examples of these speech errors that would be collected by psycholinguists are: * Anticipation: The word adds a sound from a word planned for later in the utterance. :: ''target'': paddle tennis :: ''produced'': tennis * Preservation: The word retains characteristics of a word said previously in an utterance. :: ''target'': red wagon :: ''produced'': red * Blending: More than one word is being considered in the lexicon and the two intended items "blend" into a single item. :: ''target'': shout/yell :: ''produced'': shell * Addition: Additional of linguistics material added to the word. :: ''target'': impossible :: ''produced'': * Substitution: A whole word of related meaning is replacing another. :: ''target'': at low speed it's too heavy :: ''produced'': at low speed it's too light * Malapropism: A lay term, in reference to a character Mrs. Malaprop from Sheridan's
The Rivals ''The Rivals'' is a comedy of manners by Richard Brinsley Sheridan in five acts which was first performed at Covent Garden Theatre on 17 January 1775. The story has been updated frequently, including a 1935 musical and a 1958 List of Maverick ...
, referring to the incorrect substitution of words. :: ''Makes no delusions to the past.'' :: ''The pineapple of perfection.'' :: ''I have interceded another letter from the fellow.'' * Spoonerism: The switching of the letters from two words in the utterance. :: ''target'': slips of the tongue :: ''produced'': tips of the slung


Picture-naming

Picture-naming tasks ask participants to look at pictures and name them in a certain way. By looking at the time course for the responses in these tasks, psycholinguists can learn more about the planning involved in specific phrases. These types of tasks can be helpful for investigating cross-linguistic language production and planning processes.


Elicited Production

Elicited production tasks ask participants to respond to questions or prompts in a particular way. One of the more common types of elicited production tasks is the sentence completion task. These tasks give the participants the beginning of a target sentence, which the participants are then asked to complete. Analyzing these completions can allow psycholinguistics to investigate errors that might be difficult to elicit otherwise.


Working memory involvement in language production


Working memory components

Working memory is essential for language production, consisting of three primary components: verbal, visual, and spatial. Each component serves a distinct function in processing and integrating information. Verbal working memory is central to managing phonological information, such as words and sentences, and is heavily involved in organizing syntax and lexical retrieval. Visual working memory supports the retention and manipulation of visual imagery, playing a significant role in tasks like planning written material or imagining spatial layouts. Spatial working memory contributes to the organization and sequencing of ideas, ensuring coherence and logical progression. These components often work in tandem, drawing on specialized neural networks to enable effective language production across various contexts.


Abstract versus concrete language

The nature of the language being produced—abstract or concrete—affects the engagement of verbal, visual, and spatial working memory components. Abstract language, such as metaphors or theoretical concepts, places a greater demand on verbal working memory due to the need to synthesize non-physical, conceptual relationships. It relies less on visual working memory because abstract concepts are not directly imageable. Conversely, concrete language, which describes tangible and sensory-based ideas, activates both verbal and visual working memory. Concrete ideas often enhance recall and comprehension by drawing on mental imagery, directly linking verbal and visual systems. For example, writing about familiar, imageable nouns engages visual working memory more intensively, while abstract nouns rely almost exclusively on verbal working memory. Different types of language production selectively recruit specific components of working memory.


Simple versus complex tasks

The complexity of the task directly influences working memory demands. Simple tasks like writing single words are less taxing, as they require minimal linguistic planning and retrieval.Torrance, Mark, & Nottbusch, Guido (2011). "Written Production of Single Words and Simple Sentences." In Berninger, Virginia Wise (Ed.), ''Past, Present, and Future Contributions of Cognitive Writing Research to Cognitive Psychology'' (pp. 403–422). New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-84872-963-6. However, producing complex sentences or multi-layered narratives involves greater cognitive load, engaging verbal and visual working memory for planning, syntactic integration, and coherence. Studies show that writing complex sentences can compromise fluency and coherence, as increased working memory demands make balancing retention and structure more difficult. These findings emphasize that writing is a layered cognitive process, with cognitive load increasing alongside linguistic complexity.


Written versus oral language production

Written language production is more cognitively demanding than oral production. Writing requires sustained attention, planning, encoding, and motor execution, all of which significantly tax verbal working memory. Maintaining phonological representations while managing the slower output of writing intensifies working memory load compared to the transient nature of speech. In contrast, oral production benefits from immediate feedback and conversational context, which reduce cognitive load and allow for more automatic language processing. Distinction in production processes illustrate that written and spoken language utilize overlapping but distinct cognitive resources.


Emotional States

Affect, or emotional state, plays a crucial role in language production, interacting with working memory resources to shape performance. Positive emotions enhance cognitive flexibility, aiding in idea generation and linguistic creativity.Hayes, John R. (1996-05-01). "A New Framework for Understanding Cognition and Affect in Writing". ''The Science of Writing: Theories, Methods, Individual Differences, and Applications''. Eds. C. Michael Levy & Sarah Ransdell. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1–56. ISBN 978-0805821093. Conversely, negative emotions, such as anxiety, reduce working memory capacity and disrupt fluency, particularly during complex tasks. Studies show that affect impacts not only fluency and coherence but also the tone and approach to writing, emphasizing the interconnectedness of cognitive and emotional processes in language production.


See also

* Developmental verbal dyspraxia * FOXP2 *
Langue and parole ''Langue'' and ''parole'' is a theoretical linguistic dichotomy distinguished by Ferdinand de Saussure in his '' Course in General Linguistics''. The French term ''langue'' (' n individuallanguage') encompasses the abstract, systematic rules and ...
* Speech-language pathology


References


Further reading

* {{Cite book , last1 = Carroll , first1 = David W. , title = Psychology of language , date = 2008 , publisher =
Thomson/Wadsworth Cengage Group is an American educational content, technology, and services company for higher education, K–12, professional, and library markets. It operates in more than 20 countries around the world.(June 27, 2014Global Publishing Leaders 2 ...
, location = Australia ; Belmont, CA , isbn = 978-0-495-09969-7 , oclc = 759885789 Psycholinguistics