In
linguistics
Linguistics is the scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of language, particularly its nature and structure. Lingu ...
, inversion is any of several grammatical constructions where two expressions switch their canonical order of appearance, that is, they invert. There are several types of subject-verb inversion in English: ''locative inversion'', ''directive inversion'', ''copular inversion'', and ''quotative inversion''. The most frequent type of inversion in
English
English usually refers to:
* English language
* English people
English may also refer to:
Peoples, culture, and language
* ''English'', an adjective for something of, from, or related to England
** English national id ...
is
subject–auxiliary inversion
Subject–auxiliary inversion (SAI; also called subject–operator inversion) is a frequently occurring type of inversion in English, whereby a finite auxiliary verb – taken here to include finite forms of the copula ''be'' – appears to "in ...
in which an
auxiliary verb
An auxiliary verb ( abbreviated ) is a verb that adds functional or grammatical meaning to the clause in which it occurs, so as to express tense, aspect, modality, voice, emphasis, etc. Auxiliary verbs usually accompany an infinitive verb or a ...
changes places with its
subject; it often occurs in questions, such as ''Are you coming?'', with the subject ''you'' is switched with the auxiliary ''are''. In many other languages, especially those with a freer
word order
In linguistics, word order (also known as linear order) is the order of the syntactic constituents of a language. Word order typology studies it from a cross-linguistic perspective, and examines how different languages employ different orders. C ...
than English, inversion can take place with a variety of verbs (not just auxiliaries) and with other syntactic categories as well.
When a layered
constituency-based analysis of sentence structure is used, inversion often results in the
discontinuity of a constituent, but that would not be the case with a flatter
dependency-based analysis. In that regard, inversion has consequences similar to those of
shifting.
In English
In broad terms, one can distinguish between two major types of inversion in English that involve verbs: ''subject–auxiliary inversion'' and ''subject–verb inversion''. The difference between these two types resides with the nature of the verb involved: whether it is an
auxiliary verb
An auxiliary verb ( abbreviated ) is a verb that adds functional or grammatical meaning to the clause in which it occurs, so as to express tense, aspect, modality, voice, emphasis, etc. Auxiliary verbs usually accompany an infinitive verb or a ...
or a full verb.
Subject–auxiliary inversion
The most frequently occurring type of inversion in English is subject–auxiliary inversion. The
subject and
auxiliary verb
An auxiliary verb ( abbreviated ) is a verb that adds functional or grammatical meaning to the clause in which it occurs, so as to express tense, aspect, modality, voice, emphasis, etc. Auxiliary verbs usually accompany an infinitive verb or a ...
invert (switch positions):
::a. Fred will stay.
::b. Will Fred stay?
- Subject–auxiliary inversion with yes/no question
::a. Larry has done it.
::b. What has Larry done?
- Subject–auxiliary inversion with constituent question
::a. Fred has helped at no point.
::b. At no point has Fred helped.
- Subject–auxiliary inversion with fronted expression containing negation (negative inversion
In linguistics, negative inversion is one of many types of subject–auxiliary inversion in English. A negation (e.g. ''not'', ''no'', ''never'', ''nothing'', etc.) or a word that implies negation (''only'', ''hardly'', ''scarcely'') or a phrase c ...
)
::a. If we were to surrender, ...
::b. Were we to surrender, ...
- Subject–auxiliary inversion in condition clause
The default order in English is subject–verb (SV), but a number of meaning-related differences (such as those illustrated above) motivate the subject and auxiliary verb to invert so that the finite verb precedes the subject; one ends up with auxiliary–subject (Aux-S) order. That type of inversion fails if the finite verb is not an auxiliary:
::a. Fred stayed.
::b. *Stayed Fred?
- Inversion impossible here because the verb is NOT an auxiliary verb
(The star * is the symbol used in linguistics to indicate that the example is grammatically unacceptable.)
Subject–verb inversion
In languages like Italian, Spanish, Finnish, etc. subject-verb inversion is commonly seen with a wide range of verbs and does not require an element at the beginning of the sentence. See the following Italian example:
In English, on the other hand, subject-verb inversion generally takes the form of a Locative inversion. A familiar example of subject-verb inversion from English is the
presentational ''there'' construction.
English (especially written English) also has an inversion construction involving a locative expression other than ''there'' ("in a little white house" in the following example):
Contrary to the subject-auxiliary inversion, the verb in cases of subject–verb inversion in English is not required to be an auxiliary verb; it is, rather, a full verb or a form of the copula ''be''. If the sentence has an auxiliary verb, the subject is placed after the auxiliary and the main verb. For example:
::a. A unicorn will come into the room.
::b. Into the room will come a unicorn.
Since this type of inversion generally places the focus on the subject, the subject is likely to be a full noun or noun phrase rather than a pronoun. Third-person
personal pronoun
Personal pronouns are pronouns that are associated primarily with a particular grammatical person – first person (as ''I''), second person (as ''you''), or third person (as ''he'', ''she'', ''it'', ''they''). Personal pronouns may also take dif ...
s are especially unlikely to be found as the subject in this construction:
::a. Down the stairs came the dog.
- Noun subject
::b. ''?'' Down the stairs came it.
- Third-person personal pronoun as subject; unlikely unless ''it'' has special significance and is stressed
::c. Down the stairs came I.
- First-person personal pronoun as subject; more likely, though still ''I'' would require stress
In other languages
Certain other languages, like other
Germanic languages
The Germanic languages are a branch of the Indo-European language family spoken natively by a population of about 515 million people mainly in Europe, North America, Oceania and Southern Africa. The most widely spoken Germanic language, ...
and
Romance languages
The Romance languages, sometimes referred to as Latin languages or Neo-Latin languages, are the various modern languages that evolved from Vulgar Latin. They are the only extant subgroup of the Italic languages in the Indo-European language f ...
, use inversion in ways broadly similar to English, such as in question formation. The restriction of inversion to auxiliary verbs does not generally apply in those languages; subjects can be inverted with any type of verb, but particular languages have their own rules and restrictions.
For example, in
French, ''tu aimes le chocolat'' is a declarative sentence meaning "you like the chocolate". When the order of the subject ''tu'' ("you") and the verb ''aimes'' ("like") is switched, a question is produced: ''aimes-tu le chocolat?'' ("do you like the chocolate?"). In
German
German(s) may refer to:
* Germany (of or related to)
**Germania (historical use)
* Germans, citizens of Germany, people of German ancestry, or native speakers of the German language
** For citizens of Germany, see also German nationality law
**Ger ...
, similarly, ''du magst'' means "you like", whereas ''magst du'' can mean "do you like?".
In languages with
V2 word order
In syntax, verb-second (V2) word order is a sentence structure in which the finite verb of a sentence or a clause is placed in the clause's second position, so that the verb is preceded by a single word or group of words (a single constituent) ...
, such as
German
German(s) may refer to:
* Germany (of or related to)
**Germania (historical use)
* Germans, citizens of Germany, people of German ancestry, or native speakers of the German language
** For citizens of Germany, see also German nationality law
**Ger ...
, inversion can occur as a consequence of the requirement that the verb appear as the second constituent in a declarative sentence. Thus, if another element (such as an
adverbial phrase
In linguistics, an ''adverbial phrase'' ("AdvP") is a multi-word expression operating adverbially: its syntactic function is to modify other expressions, including verbs, adjectives, adverbs, adverbials, and sentences. Adverbial phrases can be divi ...
or clause) introduces the sentence, the verb must come next and be followed by the subject: ''Ein Jahr nach dem Autounfall sieht er wirklich gut aus'', literally "A year after the car accident, looks he really good". The same occurs in some other
West Germanic languages
The West Germanic languages constitute the largest of the three branches of the Germanic family of languages (the others being the North Germanic and the extinct East Germanic languages). The West Germanic branch is classically subdivided into ...
, like
Dutch
Dutch commonly refers to:
* Something of, from, or related to the Netherlands
* Dutch people ()
* Dutch language ()
Dutch may also refer to:
Places
* Dutch, West Virginia, a community in the United States
* Pennsylvania Dutch Country
People E ...
, in which this is ''Een jaar na het auto-ongeval ziet hij er werkelijk goed uit''. (In such languages, inversion can function as a test for syntactic constituency since only one constituent may surface preverbally.)
In languages with
free word order
In linguistics, word order (also known as linear order) is the order of the syntactic constituents of a language. Word order typology studies it from a cross-linguistic perspective, and examines how different languages employ different orders. C ...
, inversion of subject and verb or of other elements of a clause can occur more freely, often for pragmatic reasons rather than as part of a specific grammatical construction.
Locative inversion
Locative inversion is a common linguistic phenomenon that has been studied by linguists of various theoretical backgrounds.
In multiple
Bantu languages
The Bantu languages (English: , Proto-Bantu: *bantʊ̀) are a large family of languages spoken by the Bantu people of Central, Southern, Eastern africa and Southeast Africa. They form the largest branch of the Southern Bantoid languages.
T ...
, such as
Chichewa
Chewa (also known as Nyanja, ) is a Bantu language spoken in much of Southern, Southeast and East Africa, namely the countries of Malawi , where it is an official language, and Mozambique and Zambia. The noun class prefix ''chi-'' is used for l ...
, the locative and subject arguments of certain verbs can be inverted without changing the
semantic roles of those arguments, similar to the English subject-verb inversion examples above. Below are examples from
Zulu, where the numbers indicate
noun classes
In linguistics, a noun class is a particular category of nouns. A noun may belong to a given class because of the characteristic features of its referent, such as gender, animacy, shape, but such designations are often clearly conventional. Some a ...
, SBJ = subject agreement prefix, APPL =
applicative suffix, FV = final vowel in Bantu verbal morphology, and LOC is the locative
circumfix
A circumfix ( abbreviated ) (also confix or ambifix) is an affix which has two parts, one placed at the start of a word, and the other at the end. Circumfixes contrast with prefixes, attached to the beginnings of words; suffixes, attached at the ...
for
adjuncts
In brewing, adjuncts are unmalted grains (such as corn, rice, rye, oats, barley, and wheat) or grain products used in brewing beer which supplement the main mash ingredient (such as malted barley). This is often done with the intention of cutt ...
.
* Canonical word order:
* Locative inversion:
In the locative inversion example, ''isikole'', "school" acts as the subject of the sentence while semantically remaining a locative argument rather than a subject/agent one. Moreover, we can see that it is able to trigger subject-verb
agreement Agreement may refer to:
Agreements between people and organizations
* Gentlemen's agreement, not enforceable by law
* Trade agreement, between countries
* Consensus, a decision-making process
* Contract, enforceable in a court of law
** Meeting of ...
as well, further indicating that it is the syntactic subject of the sentence.
This is in contrast to examples of locative inversion in English, where the semantic subject of the sentence controls subject-verb agreement, implying that it is a dislocated syntactic subject as well:
# Down the hill rolls the car.
# Down the hill roll the cars.
In the English examples, the verb ''roll'' agrees in number with ''cars'', implying that the latter is still the syntactic subject of the sentence, despite being in a noncanonical subject position. However, in the Zulu example of locative inversion, it is the noun ''isikole'', "school" that controls subject-verb agreement, despite not being the semantic subject of the sentence.
Locative inversion is observed in Mandarin Chinese. Consider the following sentences:
* Canonical word order
* Locative inversion
In canonical word order, the subject (''gǎngshào'' 'sentry') appears before the verb and the locative expression (''ménkǒu'' 'door') after the verb. In Locative inversion, the two expressions switch the order of appearance: it is the locative that appears before the verb while the subject occurs in postverbal position. In Chinese, as in many other languages, the inverted word order carry a
presentational function, that is, it is used to introduce new entities into discourse.
Theoretical analyses