Stages
Classification
Sumerian is aWriting system
Development
Transliteration
Depending on the context, a cuneiform sign can be read either as one of several possible logograms (each of which corresponds to a word in the Sumerian spoken language), as a phonetic syllable (V, VC, CV, or CVC), or as aHistoriography
The key to reading logosyllabicPhonology
Assumed phonological and morphological forms will be between slashes // and curly brackets , respectively, with plain text used for the standard Assyriological transcription of Sumerian. Most of the following examples are unattested. Note also that, not unlike most other pre-modern orthographies, Sumerian cuneiform spelling is highly variable, so the transcriptions and the cuneiform examples will generally show only one or at most a few common graphic forms out of many that may occur. Spelling practices have also changed significantly in the course of the history of Sumerian: the examples in the article will use the most phonetically explicit spellings attested, which usually means Old Babylonian or Ur III period spellings. except where an authentic example from another period is used. Modern knowledge of Sumerian phonology is flawed and incomplete because of the lack of speakers, the transmission through the filter of Akkadian phonology and the difficulties posed by the cuneiform script. As I. M. Diakonoff observes, "when we try to find out theConsonants
Early Sumerian is conjectured to have had at least the consonants listed in the table below. The consonants in parentheses are reconstructed by some scholars based on indirect evidence; if they existed, they were lost around the Ur III period in the late 3rd millennium BC. * a simple distribution of sixVowels
The vowels that are clearly distinguished by the cuneiform script are , , , and . Various researchers have posited the existence of more vowel phonemes such as and even and , which would have been concealed by the transmission through Akkadian, as that language does not distinguish them. That would explain the seeming existence of numerous homophones in transliterated Sumerian, as well as some details of the phenomena mentioned in the next paragraph. These hypotheses are not yet generally accepted. Phonemic vowel length has also been posited by many scholars based on vowel length in Sumerian loanwords in Akkadian,Edzard (2003: 13-14)Jagersma (2010: 56-57) occasional so-called ''plene'' spellings with extra vowel signs, and some internal evidence from alternations. However, scholars who believe in the existence of phonemic vowel length do not consider it possible to reconstruct the length of the vowels in most Sumerian words. During the Old Sumerian period, the southern dialects (those used in the cities ofStress
Sumerian stress (linguistics), stress is usually presumed to have been dynamic, since it seems to have caused vowel elisions on many occasions. Opinions vary on its placement. As argued by Bram Jagersma and confirmed by other scholars, the adaptation of Akkadian words of Sumerian origin seems to suggest that Sumerian stress tended to be on the last syllable of the word, at least in its citation form. The treatment of forms with grammatical morphemes is less clear. Many cases of Apheresis (linguistics), apheresis in forms with enclitics have been interpreted as entailing that the same rule was true of the phonological word on many occasions, i.e. that the stress could be shifted onto the enclitics; however, the fact that many of these same enclitics have allomorphs with apocopated final vowels (e.g. /‑še/ ~ /-š/) suggests that they were, on the contrary, unstressed when these allomorphs arose.Jagersma (2010: 63-67) It has also been conjectured that the frequent assimilation of the vowels of non-final syllables to the vowel of the final syllable of the word may be due to stress on it. However, a number of ''suffixes'' and ''enclitics'' consisting of /e/ or beginning in /e/ are also assimilated and reduced. In earlier scholarship, somewhat different views were expressed and attempts were made to formulate detailed rules for the effect of grammatical morphemes and compounding on stress, but with inconclusive results. Based predominantly on patterns of vowel elision, Adam Falkenstein argued that stress in monomorphemic words tended to be on the first syllable, and that the same applied without exception to reduplicated stems, but that the stress shifted onto the last syllable in a first member of a compound or idiomatic phrase, onto the syllable preceding a (final) suffix/enclitic, and onto the first syllable of the possessive enclitic /-ani/. In his view, single verbal prefixes were unstressed, but longer sequences of verbal prefixes attracted the stress to their first syllable. Jagersma has objected that many of Falkenstein's examples of elision are medial and so, while the stress was obviously not on the medial syllable in question, the examples do not show where it ''was''. Joachim Krecher attempted to find more clues in texts written phonetically by assuming that geminations, plene spellings and unexpected "stronger" consonant qualities were clues to stress placement. Using this method, he confirmed Falkenstein's views that reduplicated forms were stressed on the first syllable and that there was generally stress on the syllable preceding a (final) suffix/enclitic, on the penultimate syllable of a polysyllabic enclitic such as -/ani/, -/zunene/ etc., on the last syllable of the first member of a compound, and on the first syllable in a sequence of verbal prefixes. However, he found that single verbal prefixes received the stress just as prefix sequences did, and that in most of the above cases, another stress often seemed to be present as well: on the stem to which the suffixes/enclitics were added, on the second compound member in compounds, and possibly on the verbal stem that prefixes were added to or on following syllables. He also did not agree that the stress of monomorphemic words was typically initial and believed to have found evidence of words with initial as well as with final stress; in fact, he did not even exclude the possibility that stress was normally stem-final. Pascal Attinger has partly concurred with Krecher, but doubts that the stress was ''always'' on the syllable preceding a suffix/enclitic and argues that in a prefix sequence, the stressed syllable wasn't the first one, but rather the last one if heavy and the next-to-the-last one in other cases. Attinger has also remarked that the patterns observed may be the result of Akkadian language#Stress, Akkadian influence - either due to linguistic convergence while Sumerian was still a living language or, since the data comes from the Old Babylonian period, a feature of Sumerian as pronounced by native speakers of Akkadian. The latter has also been pointed out by Jagersma, who is, in addition, sceptical about the very assumptions underlying the method used by Krecher to establish the place of stress.Orthography
Sumerian writing expressed pronunciation only roughly. It was often morphophoneme, morphophonemic, so much of the allomorphy, allomorphic variation could be ignored. Especially in earlier Sumerian, coda consonants were also often ignored in spelling; e.g. /mung̃areš/ 'they put it here' could be written 𒈬𒃻𒌷 ''mu-g̃ar-re2''. The use of VC signs for that purpose, producing more elaborate spellings such as 𒈬𒌦𒃻𒌷𒌍 ''mu-un-g̃ar-re2-eš3'', became more common only in the Neo-Sumerian and especially in the Old Babylonian period. Conversely, an intervocalic consonant, especially at the end of a morpheme followed by a vowel-initial morpheme, was usually "repeated" by the use of a CV sign for the same consonant; e.g. 𒊬 ''sar'' "write" - 𒊬𒊏 ''sar-ra'' "written". This results in orthographic gemination that is usually reflected in Sumerological transliteration, but does not actually designate any phonological phenomenon such as length. It is also relevant in this context that, as explained #Consonants, above, many morpheme-final consonants seem to have been elided unless followed by a vowel at various stages in the history of Sumerian. These are traditionally termed wikt:Auslaut, Auslauts in Sumerology and may or may not be expressed in transliteration: e.g. the logogram 𒊮 for /šag/ > /ša(g)/ "heart" may be transliterated as ''šag4'' or as ''ša3''. Thus, when the following consonant appears in front of a vowel, it can be said to be expressed ''only'' by the next sign: for example, 𒊮𒂵 ''šag4-ga'' "in the heart" can also be interpreted as ''ša3-ga''. Of course, when a CVC sound sequence is expressed by a sequence of signs with the sound values CV-VC, that does not necessarily indicate a long vowel or a sequence of identical vowels either. To mark such a thing, so-called "plene" writings with an ''additional'' vowel sign repeating the preceding vowel were used, although that never came to be done systematically. A typical plene writing involved a sequence such as (C)V-V(-VC/CV), e.g. 𒂼𒀀 ''ama-a'' for /amaa/ < "the mother (ergative case)"). Sumerian texts vary in the degree to which they use logograms or opt for syllabic (phonetic) spellings instead: e.g. the word 𒃻 g̃ar "put" may also be written phonetically as 𒂷𒅈 ''g̃a2-ar''. They also vary in the degree to which allomorphic variation was expressed, e.g. 𒁀𒄄𒌍 ''ba-gi4-eš'' or 𒁀𒄄𒅖 ''ba-gi4-iš'' for "they returned". While early Sumerian writing was highly logographic, there was a tendency towards more phonetic spelling in the Neo-Sumerian period. Consistent syllabic spelling was employed when writing down the Emesal dialect (since the usual logograms would have been read in Emegir by default), for the purpose of teaching the language and often in recording incantations. As already mentioned, texts written in the Archaic Sumerian period are difficult to interpret, because they often omit grammatical elements andGrammar
Ever since its decipherment, research of Sumerian has been made difficult by the relative sparseness of linguistic data, the apparent lack of a closely related language, and the features of the writing system. A further oft-mentioned and paradoxical problem for the study of Sumerian is that the most numerous and varied texts written in the most phonetically explicit and precise orthography are only dated to periods when the scribes themselves were no longer native speakers and often demonstrably had less-than-perfect command of the language they were writing in; conversely, for much of the time during which Sumerian was still a living language, the surviving sources are few, unvaried and/or written in an orthography that is more difficult to interpret. Linguistic typology, Typologically, Sumerian is classified as anNominal morphology
Noun phrases
The Sumerian noun is typically a one or two-syllable root ( "eye", "house, household", 𒎏 "lady"), although there are also some roots with three syllables like "market". There are two semantically predictable grammatical genders, which have traditionally been called animate and inanimate, although these names do not express their membership exactly, as explained #Gender, below. The adjectives and other modifiers follow the noun ( "great king"). The noun itself is not inflected; rather, grammatical markers attach to the noun phrase as a whole, in a certain order. Typically, that order would be: An example may be: The possessive, plural and case markers are traditionally referred to as "suffixes", but have recently also been described as enclitics or postpositions.Johnson, Cale, 2004: ''In the Eye of the Beholder: Quantificational, Pragmatic and Aspectual Features of the *bí- Verbal Formation in Sumerian'', Dissertation. UCLA, Los Angeles. P.83-8= Gender
= The two genders have been variously called Animacy, animate and inanimate,Rubio (2007: 1329) animacy, human and non-human, or personal/person and impersonal/non-person.Foxvog (2016: 22) Their assignment is semantically predictable: the first gender includes humans and gods, while the second one includes animals, plants, non-living objects, abstract concepts, and groups of humans. Since the second gender includes animals, the use of the terms animate and inanimate is somewhat misleading and conventional, but it is most common in the literature, so it will be maintained in this article. There are some minor deviations from the gender assignment rules, for example: * The word for "statue" may be treated as animate. * Words for slaves such as "slave woman" and "head", used in its secondary sense of "slave", may be treated as inanimate. * In fable-like contexts, which occur frequently in Sumerian proverbs, animals are usually treated as animate.= Number
= The plural marker proper is (𒂊)𒉈 /-(e)ne/. It is used only with nouns of the animate gender and its use is optional. It is often omitted when other parts of the clause indicate the plurality of the referent.Jagersma (2010: 109-113) Thus, it is not used if the noun is modified by a numeral (''𒇽''𒁹𒁹𒁹 ''lu2 eš5'' "three men"). It has also been observed that until the Ur III period, the marker generally isn't used in a noun phrase in the absolutive case,Attinger (2009: 22)Sallaberger (2023: 47)Jagersma (2010: 111-112) unless this is necessary for disambiguation. Instead, the plurality of the absolutive participant is commonly expressed only by the form of the verb in the clause: e.g. ''𒇽𒁀𒀄𒀄𒌍 lu2 ba-zaḫ3-zaḫ3-eš'' "the men ran away", 𒇽𒈬𒅇𒆪𒁉𒌍 ''lu2 mu-u3-dab5-be2-eš'' "I caught the men". The plural marker is not used when referring to a group of people, because a group of people is treated as inanimate; e.g. 𒀳 ''engar'' "farmer" with no plural marker may refer to "(the group of) farmers". As the following example shows, the marker is appended to the end of the phrase, even after a relative clause: Likewise, the plural marker is usually (albeit not always) added only once when a whole series of coordinated nouns have plural reference: Another way in which a kind of plurality is expressed is by means of reduplication of the noun: 𒀭𒀭 ''dig̃ir-dig̃ir'' "gods", 𒌈𒌈 ''ib2-ib2'' "hips". However, this construction is usually considered to have a more specialized meaning, variously interpreted as totality ("all the gods", "both of my hips")Thomsen (2001: 61)Attinger (2009: 23) or distribution/separateness ("each of the gods taken separately").Jagersma (2010: 114-116) An especially frequently occurring reduplicated word, 𒆳𒆳 ''kur-kur'' "foreign lands", may have simply plural meaning, and in very late usage, the meaning of the reduplication in general might be simple plurality. At least a few adjectives (notably 𒃲 ''gal'' "great" and 𒌉 ''tur'' "small") are also reduplicated when the noun they modify has plural reference: 𒀀𒃲𒃲 ''a gal-gal'' "the great waters". In that case, the noun itself is not reduplicated. This is sometimes interpreted as an expression of simple plurality, while a minority view is that the meaning of these forms is not purely plural, but rather the same as that of noun reduplication. Two other ways of expressing plurality are characteristic only of very late Sumerian usage and have made their way into= Case
=Case markers The generally recognized case markers are: The final vowels of most of the above markers are subject to loss if they are attached to vowel-final words. In addition, there are the enclitic particles 𒈾𒀭𒈾 ''na-an-na'' meaning "without" and (𒀀)𒅗𒉆 ''(-a)-ka-nam'' -/akanam/ (in earlier Sumerian) or (𒀀)𒆤𒌍 ''(-a)-ke4-eš2'' -/akeš/ "because of" (in later Sumerian). Note that these nominal cases enter interact with the so-called ''dimensional prefixes'' of the verb that the noun modifies, producing additional meanings. While the dative and directive are in complementary distribution in the noun, they can nevertheless be distinguished when the verbal prefixes are taken into account. Likewise, whereas the meanings Inessive case, "in(to)" and Superessive case, "on(to)" are expressed by the same nominal case, they can be disambiguated by the verbal prefixes. This is explained in more detail in the section on ''#Dimensional prefixes, Dimensional prefixes.'' Additional spatial or temporal meanings can be expressed by genitive phrases like "at the head of" = "above", "at the face of" = "in front of", "at the outer side of" = "because of", etc.: The center embedding, embedded structure of the noun phrase can be further illustrated with the following phrase: Here, the first genitive morpheme (''-a(k)'') subordinates 𒋠 ''siki'' "wool" to 𒇻 ''udu'' "sheep", and the second subordinates 𒇻𒋠 ''udu siki-(a)k'' "sheep of wool" (or "woolly sheep") to 𒉺𒇻 ''sipad'' "shepherd".
Case usage The uses of the ergative and absolutive case are those typical of ergative languages. The subject of an intransitive verb such as "come" is in the same case as the object of a transitive verb such as "build", namely the so-called ''absolutive'' case. In contrast, the subject of a transitive verb has a different case, which is termed ''ergative''. This can be illustrated with the following examples: In contrast with the verbal morphology, Sumerian nominal morphology consistently follows this ergative principle regardless of tense/aspect, person and mood. Besides the general meanings of the case forms outlined above, there are many lexically determined and more or less unpredictable uses of specific cases, often Government (linguistics), governed by a certain verb in a certain sense: * The comitative is used to express:Jagersma (2010: 452-454) ** "to run away" (e.g. 𒀄 ''zaḫ3'') or to "take away" (e.g. 𒋼𒀀 ''kar'') ''from'' somebody; ** 𒍪 ''zu'' "to know/learn something ''from'' somebody"; ** 𒁲 ''sa2'' "to be equal ''to'' somebody" (but the same verb uses the directive in the phrasal verb ''si'' ...''sa2'' "be/put something in order", see ''#Phrasal verbs, Phrasal verbs''); ** the meaning "ago" in the construction 𒈬𒁕...𒋫 ''mu-da X-ta'' "X years ago" () * The directive is used to express: ** the objects of 𒍏 ''dab6'' "surround", 𒊏 ''raḫ2'' "hit", 𒋛 ''si'' "fill", 𒋳 ''tag'' "touch" ** ''𒈭 daḫ'' "add something ''to'' something" ** 𒄄 ''gi4'' in the sense "bring back something ''to'' something" ** 𒍑 ''us2'' "be next ''to'' something, follow something" ** 𒅗 ''dug4'' "say something ''about''/''concerning'' something" ( "say something ''about this''" often seems to have very vague reference, approaching the meaning "say something ''then''") * The locative with a directive verbal prefix, expressing "on(to)", is used to express: ** 𒆕 ''řu2'' "hold on ''to'' something" ** 𒄷𒈿 ''sa4'' "give (as a name)" ''to'' somebody/something ** ''𒁺 tum2'' "be fit ''for'' something" * 𒉚 sa''10'' "to barter" governs, in the sense to "to buy", the terminative to introduce the seller ''from'' whom something is bought, but in another construction it uses the locative for the thing something is bartered ''for;'' * 𒋾 ''ti'' "to approach" governs the dative. For the government of phrasal verbs, see #Phrasal verbs, the relevant section.
Pronouns
The attested personal pronouns are: The stem vowels of ''𒂷''(''𒂊'') ''g̃e26(-e)'' and ''𒂊 ze2'' are assimilated to a following case suffix containing /a/ and then have the forms ''𒂷 g̃a-'' and 𒍝 ''za-;'' e.g. 𒍝𒊏 ''za-ra'' 'to you (sg.)'. As far as demonstrative pronouns are concerned, Sumerian most commonly uses the enclitic ''𒁉 -bi'' to express the meaning "this". There are rare instances of other demonstrative enclitics such as 𒂊 ''-e'' "this", 𒊺 ''-še'' "that" and 𒊑 ''-re'' "that". The difference between the three has been explained in terms of increasing distance from the speaker or as a difference between proximity to the speaker, proximity to the listener and distance from both, akin to the Japanese pronouns#Demonstrative and interrogative pronouns, Japanese or Latin declension#Other demonstrative pronouns, Latin three-term demonstrative system.Wilcke, Claus 2013. ’Dieser Ur-Namma hier… Eine auf die Darstellung weisende Statueninschrift.’ ''Revue d’assyriologie et d’archéologie orientale'' 107: 173–186Adjectives
It is controversial whether Sumerian has adjectives at all, since nearly all stems with adjectival meaning are also attested as verb stems and may be conjugated as verbs: 𒈤 ''maḫ'' "great" > 𒎏𒀠𒈤 ''nin al-maḫ'' "the lady is great". Jagersma believes that there is a distinction in that the few true adjectives cannot be negated, and a few stems are different depending on the part of speech: 𒃲 ''gal'' "big", but 𒄖𒌌 ''gu-ul'' "be big". Furthermore, stems with adjective-like meaning sometimes occur with the nominalizing suffix /-a/, but their behaviour varies in this respect. Some stems appear to require the suffix always: e.g. 𒆗𒂵 ''kalag-ga'' "mighty", 𒊷𒂵 ''sag9-ga'' "beautiful", 𒁍𒁕 ''gid2-da'' "long" (these are verbs with adjectival meaning according to Jagersma). Some never take the suffix: e.g. 𒃲 ''gal'' "big", 𒌉 ''tur'' "small" and 𒈤 ''maḫ'' "great" (these are genuine adjectives according to Jagersma). Finally, some alternate: 𒍣 ''zid'' "right" often occurs as 𒍣𒁕 ''zid-da'' (these are pairs of adjectives and verbs derived from them, respectively, according to Jagersma). In the latter case, attempts have been made to find a difference of meaning between the forms with and without ''-a''; it has been suggested that the form with ''-a'' expresses a kind of determination, e.g. ''zid'' "righteous, true" vs ''zid-da'' "right (not left)", or restrictiveness, e.g. 𒂍𒉋 ''e2 gibil'' "a new house" vs 𒂍𒉋𒆷 ''e2 gibil-la'' "the new house (as contrasted with the old one)", "a/the newer (kind of) house" or "the newest house", as well as nominalization, e.g. ''tur-ra'' "a/the small one" or "a small thing". Other scholars have remained sceptical about the posited contrasts. A few adjectives, like 𒃲 ''gal'' "big" and 𒌉 ''tur'' "small" appear to "agree in number" with a preceding noun in the plural by reduplication; with some other adjectives, the meaning seems to be "each of them ADJ". The colour term 𒌓(𒌓) ''bar6''-''bar6'' / ''babbar'' "white" appears to have always been reduplicated, and the same may be true of 𒈪 ''gig2'' (actually ''giggig'') "black".Jagersma (2010: 270-272) To express the comparative or superlative degree, various constructions with the word 𒋛𒀀 ''dirig'' "exceed"/"excess" are used: X + locative + ''dirig-ga'' "which exceeds (all) X", ''dirig'' + X + genitive + terminative "exceeding X", lit. "to the excess of X".Adverbs and adverbial expressions
Most commonly, adverbial meanings are expressed by noun phrases in a certain case, e.g. 𒌓 ''ud-ba'' "then", lit. "at that time". There are two main ways to form an adverb of manner: * There is a dedicated adverbiative suffix 𒂠 ''-eš2'', which can be used to derive adverbs from both adjectives and nouns: 𒍣𒉈𒂠 ''zid-de3-eš2'' "rightly", "in the right way", 𒆰𒂠 ''numun-eš2'' 'as seeds', 'in the manner of seeds'. * the enclitic 𒁉 ''-bi'' can be added to an adjectival stem: 𒉋𒁉 ''gibil-bi'' "newly". This, too, is interpreted by Jagersma as a deadjectival noun with a possessive clitic in the directive case: , lit. "at its newness". For pronominal adverbs, see the section on #Pronouns, Pronouns.Numerals
Sumerian has a combination decimal and sexagesimal system (for example, 600 is 'ten sixties'), so that the Sumerian lexical numeral system is sexagesimal with 10 as a subbase. The cardinal numerals and ways of forming composite numbers are as follows:Jagersma (2010: 242-246) Ordinal numerals are formed with the suffix 𒄰𒈠 ''-kam-ma'' in Old Sumerian and 𒄰(𒈠) ''-kam(-ma)'' (with the final vowel still surfacing in front of enclitics) in subsequent periods. However, a cardinal numeral may also have ordinal meaning sometimes.Jagersma (2010: 246-250) The syntax of numerals has some peculiarities. Besides just being placed after a noun like other modifiers (𒌉𒐈 ''dumu eš5'' "three children" - which may, however, also be written 𒐈𒌉 ''3 dumu''), the numeral may be reinforced by the #Copula verb, copula (𒌉𒐈𒀀𒀭 ''dumu eš5''-''am3'', lit. "the children, being three". Finally, there is a third construction in which the possessive pronoun 𒁉 -''bi'' is added after the numeral, which gives the whole phrase a definite meaning: 𒌉𒐈𒀀𒁉 ''dumu eš5''-''a-bi'': "''the'' three children" (). The numerals 𒈫 ''min'' "two" and 𒐈 ''eš5'' "three" are also supplied with the nominalizing marker ''-a'' before the pronoun, as the above example shows. Fractions are formed with the phrase 𒅆...N...𒅅 ''igi-''N-''g̃al2'' : "one-Nth"; where 𒅅 ''g̃al2'' may be omitted. "One-half", however, is 𒋗𒊒𒀀 ''šu-ru-a'', later 𒋗𒊑𒀀 ''šu-ri-a.'' Another way of expressing fractions was originally limited to weight measures, specifically fractions of the Mina (unit), mina (𒈠𒈾 ''ma-na''): 𒑚 ''šuššana'' "one-third" (literarlly "two-sixths"), 𒑛 ''šanabi'' "two-thirds" (the former two words are of Akkadian origins), 𒑜 ''gig̃usila'' or 𒇲𒌋𒂆 ''la2 gig̃4 u'' "five-sixths" (literally "ten shekels split off (from the mina)" or "(a mina) minus ten shekels", respectively), 𒂆 ''gig̃4'' "one-sixtieth", lit. "a shekel" (since a shekel is one-sixtieth of a mina). Smaller fractions are formed by combining these: e.g. one-fifth is ''𒌋𒁹𒁹𒂆'' "12×1/60 = 1/5", and two-fifths are 𒑚𒇹𒂆 "1/3 + (4 × 1/60) = 5/15 + 1/15 = 6/15 = 2/5".Verbal morphology
General
The Sumerian finite verb distinguishes a number of grammatical mood, moods and agreement (grammar), agrees (more or less consistently) with the subject and the object in person, number and gender. The verb chain may also incorporate pronominal references to the verb's other modifiers, which has also traditionally been described as "agreement", although, in fact, such a reference ''and'' the presence of an actual modifier in the clause need not co-occur: not only 𒂍𒂠𒌈𒌈𒅆𒁺𒌦 ''e2-še3 ib2-ši-du-un'' "I'm going to the house", but also 𒂍𒂠𒉌𒁺𒌦 ''e2-še3 i3-du-un'' "I'm going to the house" and simply 𒌈𒅆𒁺𒌦 ''ib2-ši-du-un'' "I'm going to it" are possible. Hence, the term "cross-reference" instead of "agreement" has been proposed. This article will predominantly use the term "agreement". The Sumerian verb also makes a binary distinction according to a category that some regard as tense (past vs present-future), others as grammatical aspect, aspect (perfective vs imperfective), and that will be designated as TA (tense/aspect) in the following. The two members of the opposition entail different conjugation patterns and, at least for many verbs, different stems; they are theory-neutrally referred to with the Akkadians, Akkadian grammatical terms for the two respective forms – ''ḫamṭu'' "quick" and ''marû'' "slow, fat". Finally, opinions differ on whether the verb has a passive voice, passive or a middle voice and how it is expressed. It is often pointed out that a Sumerian verb does not seem to be strictly limited to only Transitive verb, transitive or only Intransitive verb, intransitive usage: e.g. the verb 𒆭 ''kur9'' can mean both "enter" and "insert / bring in", and the verb 𒌣 ''de2'' can mean both "flow out" and "pour out". This depends simply on whether an ergative participant causing the event is explicitly mentioned (in the clause and in the agreement markers on the verb). Some have even concluded that instead of speaking about intransitive and transitive ''verbs'', it may be better to speak only of intransitive and transitive ''constructions'' in Sumerian. The verbal root is almost always a monosyllable and, together with various affixes, forms a so-called verbal chain which is described as a sequence of about 15 slots, though the precise models differ. The finite verb has both prefixes and suffixes, while the non-finite verb may only have suffixes. Broadly, the prefixes have been divided in three groups that occur in the following order: ''modal prefixes'', "''conjugation prefixes''", and ''pronominal and dimensional'' prefixes. The suffixes are a future or imperfective marker /-ed-/, pronominal suffixes, and an /-a/ ending that nominalizes the whole verb chain. The overall structure can be summarized as follows: Examples using most of the above slots may be: More than one dimensional prefix may occur within the verb chain. If so, the prefixes are placed in a specific order, which is shown the section #Dimensional prefixes, ''Dimensional prefixes'' below. The "conjugation prefixes" appear to be mutually exclusive to a great extent, since the "finite" prefixes /i/~/e/- and /a/- do not appear before [mu]-, /ba/- and the sequence -/b/-+-/i/-, nor does the realization [mu] appear before /ba-/ or /b-i/. However, it is commonly assumed that the spellings ''im-, im-ma-'' and ''im-mi-'' are equivalent to + , + + and + + , respectively. According to Jagersma, the reason for the restrictions is that the "finite" prefixes /i/~/e/- and /a/- have been elided prehistorically in open syllables, in front of prefixes of the shape CV (consonant-vowel). The exception is the position in front of the locative prefix -/ni/-, the second person dative 𒊏 /-r-a/ and the second person directive 𒊑 /-r-i/, where the dominant dialect of the Old Babylonian period retains them.Modal prefixes
The modal prefixes express Modality (linguistics), modality. Some of them are generally combined with certain TAs; in other cases, the meaning of a modal prefix can depend on the TA. * /Ø-/ is the prefix of the simple indicative mood, indicative mood; in other words, the indicative is unmarked. E.g.: 𒅔𒅥 ''in-gu7'' "He ate it." * 𒉡 ''nu-'' and 𒆷 ''la-'', 𒇷 ''li-'' (𒉌 ''li2-'' in Ur III spelling) have negative mood, negative meaning and can be translated as "not". The allomorphs /la-/ and /li-/ are used before the "conjugation prefixes" 𒁀 ''ba-'' and 𒉈 ''bi2-'', respectively. A following vowel /i/ or /e/ is contracted with the preceding /u/ of ''nu-'' with compensatory lengthening (which is often graphically unexpressed): compare 𒉌𒁺 ''i3-du'' "he is walking", but /nu-i-du/ > /nuː-du/ 𒉡𒅇𒁺 ''nu''(-''u3'')''-du'' "he isn't walking". If followed by a consonant, on the other hand, the vowel of ''nu-'' appears to have been assimilated to the vowel of the following syllable, because it occasionally appears written as 𒈾 /na-/ in front of a syllable containing /a/. E.g.: 𒉡𒌦𒅥 ''nu''(-''u3'')''-un-gu7'' "He didn't eat it." * 𒄩 ''ḫa- / 𒃶 ḫe2-'' has either Precative mood, precative/Optative mood, optative meaning ("let him do X", "may you do X") or affirmative meaning ("he does this indeed"), partly depending on the type of verb. If the verbal form denotes a transitive action, precative meaning is expressed with the ''marû'' form, and affirmative with the ''ḫamṭu'' form. In contrast, if the verbal form is intransitive or stative, the TA used is always ''ḫamṭu''.Jagersma (2010: 561-564) Occasionally the precative/optative form is also used in a conditional sense of "if" or "when". According to Jagersma, the base form is 𒄩 ''ḫa-'', but in open syllables the prefix merges with a following conjugation prefix ''i3-'' into 𒃶 ''ḫe2-''. Beginning in the later Old Akkadian period, the spelling also shows assimilation of the vowel of the prefix to 𒃶 ''ḫe2-'' in front of a syllable containing /e/; in the Ur III period, there is a tendency to generalize the variant 𒃶 ''ḫe2-'', but in addition further assimilation to 𒄷 ''ḫu-'' in front of /u/ is attested and graphic expressions of the latter become common in the Old Babylonian period.Jagersma (2010: 558-561) Other scholars have contended that ''𒃶 ḫe2-'' was the only allomorph in the Archaic Sumerian period and many have viewed it as the main form of the morpheme. E.g.: 𒃶𒅁𒅥𒂊 ''ḫe2-eb-gu7-e'' "let him eat it!"; 𒄩𒀭𒅥 ''ḫa-an-gu7'' "He ate it indeed." * 𒂵 ''ga-'' has cohortative mood, cohortative meaning and can be translated as "let me/us do X" or "I will do X". Occasional phonetic spellings show that its vowel is assimilated to following vowels, producing the allomorphs written 𒄄 ''gi4''- and 𒄘 ''gu2''-. It is only used with ''ḫamṭu'' stems,Jagersma (2010: 518) but nevertheless uses personal prefixes to express objects, which is otherwise characteristic of the ''marû'' conjugation: 𒂵𒉌𒌈𒃻 ''ga-ni-ib2''-''g̃ar'' "let me put it there!".Jagersma 2010: 569-570 The plural number of the subject was not specially marked until the Old Babylonian period, during which the 1st person plural suffix began to be added: 𒂵𒉌𒌈𒃻𒊑𒂗𒉈𒂗 ''ga-ni-ib2-g̃ar-re-en-de3-en'' "let us put it there!". E.g.: 𒂵𒀊𒅥 ''ga-ab-gu7'' "Let me eat it!" * 𒅇 ''u3-'' has prospective aspect, prospective meaning ("after/when/if") and is also used as a mild imperative "Please do X". It is only used with ''ḫamṭu'' forms. In open syllables, the vowel of the prefix is assimilated to ''i3''- and ''a-'' in front of syllables containing these vowels. The prefix acquires an additional /l/ when located immediately before the stem, resulting in the allomorph 𒅇𒌌 ''u3''-''ul-''. E.g.: 𒌦𒅥 ''un-gu''''7'' "If/when he eats it..." * 𒈾 ''na-'' has Imperative mood, prohibitive / negative optative meaning ("Do not do it!"/"He must not do it!"/"May he not do it!") or affirmative meaning ("he did it indeed"), depending on the TA of verb: it almost always expresses negative meaning with the ''marû'' TA and affirmative meaning with the ''ḫamṭu'' TA. In its negative usage, it can be said to function as the negation of the precative/optative ''ḫa-''. In affirmative usage, it has been said to signal an emphatic assertion, but some have also claimed that it expresses reported speech (either "traditional orally transmitted knowledge" or someone else's words) or that it introduces following events/states to which it is logically connected ("as X happened (''na-''), so/then/therefore Y happened"). According to Jagersma and others, "negative ''na-''" and "affirmative ''na-''" are actually two different prefixes, since "negative ''na-''" has the allomorph /nan-/ before a single consonant (written 𒈾𒀭 ''na-an-'' or, in front of the labial consonants /b/ and /m/, ''𒉆 nam-''), whereas "affirmative ''na-''" does not. E.g.: 𒈾𒀊𒅥𒂊 ''na-ab-gu7-e'' "He must not eat it!"; 𒈾𒀭𒅥 ''na-an-gu7'' "He ate it indeed." * 𒁀𒊏 ''ba-ra-'' has emphatic negative meaning ("He certainly does/will not do it") or Imperative mood, vetitive meaning ("He should not do it!"),Edzard (2003: 117) although some consider the latter usage rare or non-existent. It can often function as the negation of cohortative ''ga-'' and of affirmative ''ḫa-''. It is combined with the ''marû'' TA if the verb denies an action (always present or future), and with the ''ḫamṭu'' TA if it denies a state (past, present or future) or an action (always in the past).Jagersma (2010: 574-575) The vetitive meaning requires it to be combined with the ''marû'' TA, at least if the action is transitive. E.g.: 𒁀𒊏𒀊𒅥𒂗 ''ba-ra-ab-gu7-en'' "I certainly will not eat it!"; 𒁀𒊏𒀭𒅥 ''ba-ra-an-gu7'' "He certainly didn't eat it." * 𒉡𒍑 ''nu-uš-'' is a rare prefix that has been interpreted as having "frustrative" meaning, i.e. as expressing an unrealizable wish ("If only he would do it!"). It occurs both with ''ḫamṭu'' and with ''marû.'' E.g.: 𒉡𒍑𒌈𒅥𒂊 ''nu''-''uš''-''ib2-gu7-e "''If only he would eat it!" * ''𒅆 ši-'', earlier 𒂠 ''še3-'', is a rare prefix, with unclear and disputed meaning, which has been variously described as affirmative ("he does it indeed"),Jagersma (2010: 578-579), citing Falkenstein. contrapunctive ("correspondingly", "on his part"), as "reconfirming something that already ha(s) been stated or ha(s) occurred", or as "so", "therefore". It occurs both with ''ḫamṭu'' and with ''marû.'' In Southern Old Sumerian, the vowel alternated between /e/ before open vowels and /i/ before close ones in accordance with the vowel harmony rule of that dialect; later, it displays assimilation of the vowel in an open syllable, depending on the vowel of the following syllable, to /ša-/ (𒊭 ''ša-'' / 𒁺 ''ša4-'') and (first attested in Old Babylonian) to 𒋗 ''šu-''.Edzard (2003: 120) E.g.: 𒅆𒅔𒅥 ''ši-in-gu7'' "So/correspondingly/accordingly(?), he ate it." Although the modal prefixes are traditionally grouped together in one slot in the verbal chain, their behaviour suggests a certain difference in status: only ''nu-'' and ''ḫa-'' exhibit morphophonemic evidence of co-occurring with a following finite "conjugation prefix", while the others do not and hence seem to be mutually exclusive with it. For this reason, Jagersma separates the first two as "Clitic, proclitics" and groups the others together with the finite prefix as (non-proclitic) "preformatives"."Conjugation prefixes"
The meaning, structure, identity and even the number of the various "conjugation prefixes" have always been a subject of disagreements. The term "conjugation prefix" simply alludes to the fact that a Sumerian finite verb in the indicative mood must (nearly) always contain one of them. ''Which'' of these prefixes is used seems to have, more often than not, no effect on its translation into European languages. Proposed explanations of the choice of conjugation prefix usually revolve around the subtleties of spatial grammar, information structure (Focus (linguistics), focus), verb valency, and, most recently, grammatical voice, voice.' The following description primarily follows the analysis of Jagersma (2010), largely seconded by Zólyomi (2017) and Sallaberger (2023), in its specifics; nonetheless, most of the interpretations in it are held widely, if not universally. * ''𒉌 i3-'' (Southern Old Sumerian ''variant: 𒂊 e-'' in front of open vowels), sometimes described as a finite prefix, appears to have a neutral Finite verb, finite meaning.Jagersma (2010: 535-542) As mentioned above, it generally does not occur in front of a prefix or prefix sequence of the shape CV except, in Old Babylonian Sumerian, in front of the locative prefix 𒉌 -/ni/-, the second person dative 𒊏 -/r-a/- and the second person directive 𒊑 -/r-i/-.'' E.g.: 𒅔𒁺 ''in-ře6'' "He brought (it)." * ''𒀀 a-'', with the variant ''𒀠 al-'' used in front of the stem, the other finite prefix, is rare in most Sumerian texts outside of the imperative form, but when it occurs, it usually has Stative verb, stative meaning.Cf. Thomsen (2001: 187), Edzard (2003: 111-112), Foxvog (2016: 66), Rubio (2007: 1351). It is common in the Northern Old Sumerian dialect, where it can also have a Passive voice, passive meaning. According to Jagersma, it was used in the South as well during the Old Sumerian period, but only in subordinate clauses, where it regularly characterized not only stative verbs in ''ḫamṭu'', but also verbs in ''marû''; in the Neo-Sumerian period, only the pre-stem form ''al-'' was still used and it no longer occurred with ''marû'' forms. Like ''i3-'', the prefix ''a-'' does not occur in front of a CV sequence except, in Old Babylonian Sumerian, in front of the locative prefix ''𒉌'' -/ni/-, the second person dative 𒊏 -/r-a/- and the second person directive 𒊑 -/r-i/-''.'' E.g.: 𒀠𒁺 ''al-ře6'' "It is/was brought." * 𒈬 ''mu-'' is most commonly considered to be a Andative and venitive, ventive prefix, expressing movement towards the speaker or proximity to the speaker; in particular, it is an obligatory part of the 1st person dative form 𒈠 ''ma-'' (''mu- + -a-''). However, many of its occurrences appear to express more subtle and abstract nuances or general senses, which different scholars have sought to pinpoint. They have often been derived from "abstract nearness to the speaker" or "involvement of the speaker". It has been suggested, variously, that ''mu-'' may be adding nuances of emotional closeness or alignment of the speaker with the agent or other participants of the event, Topic (linguistics), topicality, foregrounding of the event as something essential to the message with a Focus (linguistics), focus on a person, movement or action directed towards an entity with higher social status, prototypical Transitivity (grammar), transitivity with its close association with "control, agency, and animacy" as well as focus or emphasis on the role of the agent, telicity as suchFoxvog (2016: 94-95) or that it is attracted by personal dative prefixes in general, as is the Akkadian ventive. E.g. 𒈬𒌦𒁺 ''mu-un-ře6'' "He brought it here." * 𒅎 ''im-'' and ''𒀀𒀭am3-'' are widely seen as being formally related to ''mu-'' and as also having ventive meaning; according to Jagersma, they consist of an allomorph of ''mu-'', namely -/m/-, and the preceding prefixes ''𒉌 i3-'' and ''𒀀 a-''. In his analysis, these combinations occur in front of a CV sequence, where the vowel ''-u-'' of ''mu-'' is lost, whereas the historically preceding finite prefix is preserved: */i-mu-ši-g̃en/ > 𒅎𒅆𒁺 ''im-ši-g̃en'' "he came for it". In Zólyomi's slightly different analysis, which is supported by Sallaberger, there may also be a -/b/- in the underlying form, which also elicits the allomorph -/m/-: * > /i-m-b-ši-g̃en/ > /i-m-ši-g̃en/. The vowel of the finite prefix undergoes compensatory lengthening immediately before the stem */i-mu-g̃en/ > 𒉌𒅎𒁺 ''i3-im-g̃en'' "he came".Jagersma (2010: 530, 499) E.g. 𒅎𒁺𒈬 ''im-tum3-mu'' "He will bring it here." * The vowel of ''mu-'' is ''not'' elided in front of the locative prefix ''𒉌 -ni-'', the second person dative 𒊏 /-r-a/ and the second person directive 𒊑 /-r-i/. It may, however, be assimilated to the vowel of the following syllable. This produces two allomorphs: ** 𒈪 ''mi-'' in the sequences 𒈪''𒉌 mi-ni-'' and 𒈪𒊑 ''mi-ri-''. E.g. 𒈪𒉌𒅔𒁺 ''mi-ni-in-ře6'' "He brought it in here." ** 𒈠 ''ma-'' in the sequence 𒈠𒊏 ''ma-ra-''. E.g. 𒈠𒊏𒀭𒁺 ''ma-ra-an-ře6'' "He brought (it) here to you." * 𒉈 ''bi2-'' (Old Sumerian Lagaš spelling: 𒁉 ''bi-'' or ''be2-'' in front of open vowels; Old Sumerian Ur spelling: 𒉿 ''be6-'') is usually seen as a sequence of the personal prefix -/b/-Jagersma (2010: 417) and the directive prefix -/i/- or -/e/-. E.g. 𒉈𒅔𒁺 ''bi2-in-ře6'' "He made it (the ox, the group of workers) bring (it)." * 𒁀 ''ba-'' can be analysed as a sequence of the personal prefix /b/- and the dative prefix -/a/-.Jagersma (2010: 400-401) However, it has been argued that, in spite of this origin, /ba-/ now occupies a slot of its own before the first pronominal prefix and the dimensional prefixes. In accordance with its assumed origin as ''b-a-'', it has often been observed that ''ba-'' appears to have the meaning of a "3rd person inanimate dative": "for it", "to it". However, this explains only some of its occurrences. A number of other apparent meanings and uses of ''ba-'' have been noted, and most of these are subsumed by Jagersma under the overarching function of a Voice (grammar), middle voice marker.Jagersma (2010: 487-496)Cf. Edzard (2003: 95), Woods (2008: 303), Civil (2020: 172, 176). Foxvog (2016: 75), Thomsen (2001: 183) and Rubio (2007: 1349) dispute the accuracy of the term, but nonetheless acknowledge the tendency of ''ba-'' to occur in the absence of an (explicit) agent. Both Rubio and Thomsen view it as being in some sense the opposite of ''mu-'' (as does Woods): according to Rubio (2007: 347-1348), ''ba-'' expresses "focus on locus" as opposed to person; according to Thomsen (2001: 179), it is "preferred with inanimate and non-agentive subjects" and, at least in early Neo-Sumerian texts, before case prefixes referring to inanimate beings. They include: *# a Reflexive pronoun, reflexive Object (grammar), indirect object (to do something "for oneself"); *# separation and movement "away" from the centre of attention towards a distant goal, especially with motion verbs; *# a change of state;Jagersma (2010: 487-494) *# the passive voice, i.e. occurrence with normally transitive verbs when their agent is not mentioned (the latter not in Northern Sumerian according to Jagersma). Some researchers also view it more generally as expressing focus or emphasis on the patient/goal and relatively low transitivity (and thereby as the polar opposite of ''mu-'' as they understand it). E.g. 𒁀𒀭𒁺 ''ba-an-ře6'' "He brought it to it" / "He took it for himself" / "He took it away"; 𒁀𒁺 ''ba-ře6'' "It was brought." * 𒅎𒈪 ''im-mi-'' (Southern Old Sumerian ''𒉌𒈪 i3-mi'' or, in front of open vowels, 𒂊𒈨 ''e-me-'') and 𒅎𒈠 ''im-ma-'' (Southern Old Sumerian 𒂊𒈠 ''e-ma-'') are generally seen as closely related to one another and ''im-mi-'' is widely considered to contain the directive prefix ''-i~e-''. One common analysis is that ''im-mi-'' and ''im-ma-'' represent sequences of ''im-'' and ''bi2-'' and ''ba-'', respectively, where the consonant /b/ has undergone assimilation to the preceding /m/. Accordingly, their meaning is considered to be simply a combination of the ventive meaning of ''im-'' and the meanings of ''bi2-'' and ''ba-'', on which see above. This is the analysis espoused by Jagersma and Zólyomi and it is reflected in the schemes and examples in this article. Alternatively, some authors regard ''im-ma-'' as a prefix in its own right, and it has sometimes been ascribed a Voice (grammar), middle voice meaning distinct from the more Passive voice, passive nuance of ''ba-''. E.g. 𒅎𒈪𒅔𒁺 ''im-mi-in-ře6'' "He made it (the ox, the group of workers) bring it here"; 𒅎𒈠𒁺 ''im-ma''-''ře6'' "It was brought here." * ''𒀀𒀭𒈪 am3-mi-'' and ''𒀀𒀭𒈠 am3-ma-'' are typically analysed along the same lines as ''im-mi-'' and ''im-ma-'', but with a preceding ''am-'' (from ''a-'') instead of ''im-'' (from ''i-''); on the meaning of these see above. The rare prefix -/nga/- means 'also', 'equally' (often written without the initial /n/, especially in earlier periods). It is of crucial importance for the ordering of the "conjugation prefixes", because it is usually placed between the conjugation prefix /i/- and the pronominal prefix, e.g. 𒅔𒂵𒀭𒍪 ''in-ga-an-zu'' 'he, too, knows it', but it precedes the conjugation prefix /mu/-: 𒈾𒂵𒈬𒍪 ''na-ga-mu-zu'' 'he also understood it'. This suggests that these two conjugation prefixes must belong to different slots. Although a conjugation prefix is almost always present, Sumerian until the Old Babylonian period allows a finite verb to begin directly with the locative prefix -/ni/-, the second person singular dative -/r-a/-, or the second person directive -/r-i/- (see below), because the prefixes ''i3-''/''e-'' and ''a-'' are apparently elided in front of them.Pronominal and dimensional prefixes
The ''dimensional prefixes'' of the verb chain basically correspond to, and often repeat, the case markers of the noun phrase. Like the case markers of the noun phrase, the first dimensional prefix is normally attached to a preceding "head" – a ''pronominal prefix'', which expresses the person, gender and number of its referent. The first dimensional prefix may be followed by up to two other dimensional prefixes, but unlike the first one, these prefixes never have an explicit "head" and cannot refer to animate nouns. The other slot where a pronominal prefix can occur is immediately before the stem, where it can have a different allomorph and expresses the person, gender and absolutive or the ergative participant (the transitive subject, the intransitive subject or the direct object), depending on the TA and other factors, as explained #Pronominal agreement with subjects and direct objects, below. There is some variation in the extent to which the verb of a clause that contains a noun in a given case also contains the corresponding pronominal and dimensional prefixes in the verb. The ergative participant is always expressed in the verb, as is, generally, the absolutive one (with some vacillation for the third person singular inanimate in transitive forms, as explained #Pronominal agreement with subjects and direct objects, below); the dative, comitative, the locative and directive participant (used in a local meaning) also tend to be expressed relatively consistently; with the ablative and terminative, on the other hand, there is considerable variability.Zólyomi (2017: 86) There are some cases, specified #Dimensional prefixes, below, where the meanings of the cases in the noun phrase and in the verb diverge, so a noun case enclitic may not be reflected in the verb or, conversely, a verb may have a prefix that has no specific reference in the clause or in reality.= Pronominal prefixes
= The forms of the pronominal prefixes are the following: Confusingly, the subject and object prefixes (/-n-/, /-b-/, /-e-/, /-V-/) are not commonly spelled out in early texts, as both coda consonants and vowel length are often ignored in them. The "full" spellings do become more usual during the= Dimensional prefixes
= The generally recognized dimensional prefixes are shown in the table below; if several occur within the same verb complex, they are placed in the order they are listed in. The ablative does not co-occur with the terminative, and the directive does not co-occur with the locative, so these pairs may be argued to share the same slot. Accordingly, the template can be said to include the following dimensional slots: dative - comitative - ablative/terminative - directive/locative. A major exception from the general system of personal and dimensional prefixes is the very frequent prefix 𒉌 ''-ni-'' "(in) there", which corresponds to a noun phrase in the locative, but doesn't seem to be preceded by any pronominal prefix and has demonstrative meaning by itself. This prefix is not to be confused with the homographic sequence 𒉌 ''-ni-'' which corresponds to an animate noun phrase in the directive. In the latter case, ''ni'' is analysed as a combination of pronominal /-nn-/ and directive /-i-/ (roughly: "at him/her", "on him/her", etc.), whereas in the former, ''ni'' is unanalysable. An example of a verb chain where several dimensional slots are occupied can be: The comitative prefix ''-da-'' can, in addition, express the meaning "to be able to". In that case, there is a preceding pronominal prefix agreeing with the subject of the action: e.g. "you cannot catch him" (). The directive has the meaning "on(to)" when the verb is combined with a noun in the locative case: e.g. "I will put bread on the table".= Differences and combinations between dimensional prefixes and noun case markers
= While the meanings of the prefixes are generally the same as those of the corresponding nominal case markers, there are some differences: * The prefixes, unlike noun phrases in the corresponding cases, normally refer only to participants with a strong relationship to the action or state expressed by the verb (e.g. a temporal meaning like ''since X'' may be expressed by means of a noun phrase with a ''-ta'' case marker, but that normally wouldn't be cross-referenced with a ''-ta'' prefix on the verb).Jagersma (2010: 392-396, 458-459, 474) * The use of dimensional prefixes is sometimes more closely connected to special meanings of specific verbs and to lexical idiosyncrasies. For instance, the verb 𒇯𒁺 ''ed3'' has the meaning "go up" with the directive prefix, but "go down" with the ablative one, the verb 𒉚 ''sa10'' means "sell" with the ablative prefix and "buy" with the terminative, the verb 𒌓𒁺 ''ed2'' "leave, go out" always has the ablative prefix, and the phrasal verb 𒅗 ... 𒄄 ''inim ... gi4'' "answer" () always includes the locative. In general, verbs having a place-related meaning such as 𒁄 ''bala'' "cross", 𒅅 ''g̃al2'' "be (somewhere), 𒃻 ''g̃ar'' "put", 𒁺 ''gub'' "stand", 𒆭 ''kur9'' "enter", 𒋛 ''sig9'' "put" and 𒆪 ''tuš'' "sit" generally occur with a dimensional prefix specifying a location.Jagersma (2010: 392-394) Thus, a verb may, albeit rarely, contain a dimensional prefix that simply modifies its meaning and has no reference. In such cases, it has no preceding pronominal prefix, even if it is the first dimensional prefix: e.g. 𒁀𒊏𒀭𒉚 ''ba-ra-an-sa10'' "he sold it". * The directive may be replaced by the dative when its slot is occupied by the locative or when it would have had animate reference, but there is a preceding prefix, which makes any further prefixes with animate reference illicit. At the systemic level, there are some asymmetries between the nominal case markers and the verbal dimensional prefixes: they partly make different distinctions, and the nominal case marking is influenced by animacy. Because of these mismatches, different meanings are expressed by combinations of matching or non-matching noun cases and verb prefixes. The combinations may be summarized as follows: In some cases, there are also mismatches between nominal and verbal markers when exact correspondences would have been possible;Jagersma (2010: 396)Foxvog (2016: 69-70). Cf. Zólyomi (2017: 86-87), who does not mention such a possibility. Jagersma (2010) interprets such apparent absences of case markers mostly as orthographic omissions of consonant-final allomorphs. these may serve to express additional shades of meaning. A dative noun case marker and terminative dimensional prefix may co-occur in the Ur III period. In general, from that time on, the choice of noun cases begins to be influenced by the government of corresponding Akkadian verbs, while the verbs themselves retain their older prefixes. According to Foxvog, /-ni-/ can resume non-locative cases such as the terminative and the dative. A peculiar pattern of agreement occurs in what has been referred to as an ''external possession construction'', in which a modifier of the verb refers to a certain object, almost always a body part, but it is emphasised that the action affects the ''possessor'' of that object (cf. English "he hit ''me'' on the head"). In that case, the verb may agree with the possessor with the directive prefix, while not agreeing with the object itself: thus, "he put barley in your hand" may be expressed by , lit. "he put barley at you, in your hand". Alternatively, it may agree with both the possessor and the object: the possessor is then referred to by the dative prefix: , lit. "he put barley to you, in there, in your hand".Use of the ventive as a 1st person marker When the dimensional prefix is dative -/a/-, the personal prefix of the 1st person appears to be absent, but the 1st person reference is expressed by the choice of the ventive conjugation prefix /mu/-''.'' The sequence that expresses the 1st person dative is then: /mu-/ + /-a-/ → 𒈠 ''ma-''.Jagersma (2010: 388, 508-509)Zólyomi (2017: 81)Rubio 2007 When the intended meaning is that of the directive -/i/~/e/- ("on me", "in contact with me", etc.), it seems that the ventive conjugation prefix 𒈬 ''mu-'' alone serves to express it.
Syncope of /i/ in -/ni/- and -/bi/- Two special phenomena occur if there is no absolutive–ergative pronominal prefix in the pre-stem position. 1. The sequences 𒉌 -/ni/- (locative and personal + directive ) and 𒉈 /bi/- (personal + directive ) acquire the forms -/n/- and -/b/- (coinciding with the ''absolutive–ergative'' pronominal prefixes) before the stem if there isn't already an absolutive–ergative pronominal prefix in pre-stem position. This is typically the case when the verb is used intransitively. For example, the normal appearance of ''-ni-'' is seen in: * "he brought (it) in" () to go in)' > /muninkur/, written 𒈬𒉌𒆭 ''mu-ni-kur9'' in early texts, later 𒈬𒉌𒅔𒆭 ''mu-ni-in-kur9''. In contrast, in an intransitive form, we find a Syncope (phonology), syncopated realization: * "he went in" > /muːnkur/, written 𒈬𒆭 ''mu-kur9'' in early texts, later 𒈬𒌦𒆭 ''mu-un-kur9''. The preceding vowel undergoes compensatory lengthening, which is sometimes indicated by its doubling in the spelling: * > ''i3-in-kur9'' 𒉌𒅔𒆭 "he went in". Likewise, the normal realisation of ''bi-'' is seen in: * > ''bi2-in-si'' 𒉈𒅔𒋛 "he loaded (it) on it". This is to be contrasted with the syncopated version in an intransitive form: * > ''i3-ib2-si'' 𒉌𒌈𒋛 "(it) was loaded on it". The same phonological pattern is claimed to account for the alternation between the forms of the ventive prefix. The standard appearance is seen in: > ''mu-un-ak'' 𒈬𒌦𒀝 "he did it here". In an intransitive form, however, we find: > ''i3-im-g̃en 𒉌𒅎𒁺'' "he came here".
Expression of the directive by a pre-stem personal prefix A superficially very similar, but distinct phenomenon is that if there isn't already an absolutive–ergative pronominal prefix in pre-stem position, the personal prefix of the directive participant does not receive the dimensional prefix -/i/~/e/- at all and is moved to the pre-stem position. For example, the normal position of the directive participant is seen in: * ''bi2-in-ak'' 𒉈𒅔𒀝 "he applied (it) to it" (said of oil). In contrast, in an intransitive form, we find: * ''ba-ab-ak'' 𒁀𒀊𒀝 "it was applied to it". In the same way, the normal position is seen in: * ''bi2-in-us2'' 𒉈𒅔𒍑 ≈ "he adjoined (it) to it". This can be contrasted with an intransitive form: * ''ib2-us2'' 𒌈𒍑 ≈ "(it) was adjoined to it".
Absence of In some cases, the 3rd person inanimate prefix ''-b-'' appears to be unexpectedly absent. * ''-b-'' as the head of a dimensional prefix isn't used after the "conjugation prefix" ''ba-'': thus *𒁀𒀊𒅆𒌈𒄄𒄄 ''ba-ab-ši-ib2-gi4-gi4'' "he will return it to it (for himself)" is impossible. This restriction does not, however, apply for ''-b-'' as a subject/object prefix immediately before the stem: thus, 𒁀𒀊𒄄𒄄 ''ba-ab-gi4-gi4'' "he will return it (for himself)" is possible. In some schemes, this is formalized as the placement of the initial pronominal prefix ''b-'' in the same slot as ''ba-'' and not in the following slot, where all the other initial pronominal prefixes such as ''-n-'' are located. * ''-b-'' also regularly "fails" to appear after the ventive "conjugation prefix" ''mu-'': instead of expected *𒈬𒌒𒅆𒁺 ''mu-ub-ši-g̃en'', the meaning "he came for it" is expressed by 𒅎𒅆𒁺 ''im-ši-g̃en.'' Similarly, instead of *𒈬𒌒𒂷𒂷 ''mu-ub-g̃a2-g̃a2'' for "he is placing it here", we find 𒉌𒅎𒂷𒂷 ''i3-im-g̃a2-g̃a2''. While some believe that /b/ in this case is truly omitted, others assume that such forms in fact contain an assimilated sequence -/mb/- > -/mm/- > -/m/-, just like the forms ''im-mi-'' and ''im-ma-'', so that the above realisations actually stand for and .Zólyomi (2017: 151-155) * For another case of absence of ''-b-'', see the footnote on -''b''- as a marker of the transitive object in the table in the section on ''Pronominal agreement in conjugation''.
Pronominal suffixes
The pronominal suffixes are as follows: The initial vowel in all of the above suffixes can be assimilated to the vowel of the verb root; more specifically, it can become /u/ or /i/ if the vowel of the verb root is /u/ or /i/, respectively. It can also undergo contraction with an immediately preceding vowel. Pre-Ur III texts also spell the first- and second-person suffix -/en/ as -/e/, making it coincide with the third person in the ''marû'' form.Pronominal agreement with subjects and direct objects
Sumerian verbal agreement follows a nominative–accusative language, nominative–accusative pattern in the 1st and 2nd persons of the ''marû'' Grammatical tense, tense-Grammatical aspect, aspect, but an ergative–absolutive language, ergative–absolutive pattern in most other forms of the indicative mood. Because of this presence of both patterns, Sumerian is considered a language with split ergativity.Zólyomi (2017: 125) The general principle is that in the ''ḫamṭu'' TA, the transitive subject is expressed by the prefix, and the direct object by the suffix, and in the ''marû'' TA it is the other way round. For example, can be a ''ḫamṭu'' form meaning "it caught me", where expresses the subject "it" and expresses the object "I". However, it can also be a ''marû'' form meaning "I will catch it", where expresses the subject "I" and expresses the object "it". As for the intransitive subject, it is expressed, in both TAs, by the suffixes. For example, is "I ran", and can be "I will run". This means that the intransitive subject is treated like the object in ''ḫamṭu'' (which makes the ''ḫamṭu'' pattern ergative) and like the subject in ''marû'' (which makes the ''marû'' pattern nominative-accusative). There are two exceptions from the above generalization: 1. A transitive subject of the ''third'' person in ''marû'' uses unique suffixes that are ''not'' the same as those of the intransitive subject and the ''ḫamṭu'' direct object. For example, while "they ran" can be , just as "it caught them" can be , the corresponding form for "they will catch it" would be . This pattern can be described as a case of tripartite alignment. 2. A plural transitive subject in the ''ḫamṭu'' TA is expressed not only by the prefix, but also by the suffix: e.g. can mean "they caught (it)". Specifically, the prefix expresses only the person, while the suffix expresses both the person and the number of the subject. Note that the prefixes of the plural transitive subject are identical to those of the singular – -/V/-, -/e/-, -/n/- – as opposed to the special plural forms ''-me-'', ''-e-ne-'', ''-ne-'' found in non-pre-stem position. The use of the personal affixes for subjects and direct objects can be summarized as follows: Examples for TA and pronominal agreement: (''ḫamṭu'' is rendered with past tense, ''marû'' with present): * (𒉌𒁺𒁉𒂗): "I stood" or "I stand" * (𒅔𒁺𒁉𒂗): "he placed me" or "I place him" * (𒉌𒁻𒂗𒉈𒂗): "we stood/stand" * (𒅔𒁶𒂗𒉈𒂗): "he created us" or "we create him" * (𒈬𒁶𒂗𒉈𒂗): "we created [someone or something]" * (𒌈𒁺𒁉) "he places it" * (𒌈𒁶𒈨''𒉈''): "they create it" * (𒅔𒁶𒈨𒌍): "they created [someone or something]" or "he created them" * (𒉌𒁻𒄀𒌍): "they stood" or "they stand".Stem
The verbal stem itself can also express grammatical distinctions within the categories number and tense-aspect. In a number of verbs, this involves suppletion or morphonological alternations that are not fully predictable. 1. With respect to number, plurality can be expressed by ''complete'' reduplication of the ''ḫamṭu'' stem (e.g. 𒆭𒆭 ''kur9-kur9'' "enter (pl.)" or by a suppletive stem (e.g. 𒁺 ''gub'' "stand (sing.)" - 𒁻 ''sug2'' "stand (pl.)". The traditional view is that both of these morphological means express plurality of the absolutive participant in Sumerian.Rubio (2007: 1338)Thomsen (2001: 125) However, it has often been pointed out that complete reduplication of the verb in Sumerian can also express "plurality of the action itself" intensity or iterative, iterativity, and that it is not obligatory in the presence of plural participants, but rather seems to expressly emphasize the plurality. According to some researchers, the predominant meaning of the suppletive plural stem is, indeed, plurality of the most affected participants, whereas the predominant meaning of complete reduplication is plurality of events (because they occur at multiple times or locations). However, even with suppletive plural stems, the singular may occur with a plural participant, presumably because the event is perceived as a single one. 2. With respect to tense-aspect marking, verbs are divided in four types; ''ḫamṭu'' is always the unmarked TA. * The stems of the 1st type, regular verbs, are analysed in two ways: some scholars believe that they do not express TA at all, while others claim that they express ''marû'' TA by adding a suffix -/e/ as in 𒁶𒂊 ''dim2-e'' vs 𒁶 ''dim2'' "make". This -/e/ would, however, nowhere be distinguishable from the first vowel of the pronominal suffixes except for intransitive ''marû'' 3rd person singular; in that last form, the first analysis attributes the -/e/ to the presence of the -/e(d)/ suffix described #The modal or imperfective suffix -/ed/, below. The glosses in this article assume the first analysis. * The 2nd type expresses ''marû'' by ''partial'' reduplication of the stem, e.g. 𒆭 ''kur9'' vs 𒆭𒆭 ''ku4-ku4'' "enter". Usually, as in this example, this ''marû'' reduplication follows the pattern C1V1-C1V1 (C1 = 1st consonant of the root, V = 1st vowel of the root). In a few cases, the template is instead C1V1C1C2V1.Jagersma (2010: 312-314) * The 3rd type expresses ''marû'' by adding a consonant, e.g. ''te'' vs ''teg̃3'' "approach" (both written 𒋼). A number of scholars do not recognise the existence of such a class or consider it dubious. * The 4th type uses a suppletive stem, e.g. 𒅗 ''dug4'' vs 𒂊 ''e'' "do, say". Thus, as many as four different suppletive stems can exist, as in the admittedly extreme case of the verb "to go": 𒁺 ''g̃en'' ("to go", ''ḫamṭu'' sing.), 𒁺 ''du'' (''marû'' sing.), (𒂊)𒁻 (''e-'')''re7'' (''ḫamṭu'' plur.), 𒁻 ''sub2'' (''marû'' plur.). The following tables show some of the most frequent stem alternations.The modal or imperfective suffix -/ed/
Before the pronominal suffixes, a suffix -/ed/ or -/d/ can be inserted (the /d/ is only realized if other vowels follow, in which case the /e/ in turn may be elided): e.g. 𒉌𒀄(𒂊)𒉈𒂗 ''i3-zaḫ3(-e)-de3-en'' "I will/must escape", 𒉌𒀄𒂊 ''i3-zaḫ3-e'' "he will/must escape". This suffix is considered to account for occurrences of ''-e'' in the third-person singular ''marû'' of intransitive forms by those who do not accept the theory that ''-e'' itself is a ''marû'' stem formant.Zólyomi 2005 The function of the suffix is somewhat controversial. Some view it as having a primarily modal meaning of "must" or "can" or future meaning. Others believe that it primarily signals simply the imperfective status of a verb form, i.e. a ''marû'' form, although its presence is obligatory only in intransitive ''marû'' forms and in non-finite forms. In intransitive forms, it thus helps to distinguish ''marû'' from ''ḫamṭu''; for instance, in the above example, 𒉌𒀄𒂗 ''i3-zaḫ3-en'' alone, without -/ed/-, could have been interpreted as a ''ḫamṭu'' form "I escaped". In contrast, in the analysis of scholars who do not believe that -/ed/- is obligatory in ''marû'', many intransitive forms like ''i3-zaḫ3-en'' can be both ''ḫamṭu'' and ''marû''. The vowel /e/ of this suffix undergoes the same allophonic changes as the initial /e/ of the person suffixes. It is regularly assimilated to /u/ in front of stems containing the vowel /u/ and a following labial consonant, /r/ or /l/, e.g. 𒋧𒈬𒁕 ''šum2-mu(-d)'' (< ). It is also assimilated and contracted with immediately preceding vowels, e.g. 𒄄 ''gi4-gi4'' /gi-gi-i(d)/ < "which will/should return". The verb 𒁺 ''du'' "go" never takes the suffix.Use of the tense-aspect forms
Jagersma systematizes the use of the tense-aspect forms in the following patterns: * ''ḫamṭu'' is used to express completed (perfective aspect, perfective) actions in the past, but also states (past ''or'' present) and timeless truths. It is also used in conditional clauses with the conjunction 𒋗𒃻𒌉𒇲𒁉 ''tukumbi'' 'if'. * ''marû'' is used to express actions in the present and future, but also non-completed (imperfective aspect, imperfective) actions in the past (like the English past progressive tense), and, rarely, actions in the past that are still relevant or operative (like the English present perfect tense). It is also used in conditional clauses with the conjunction 𒌓𒁕 ''ud-da'' 'if'. Verbum dicendi, Verba dicendi introducing direct speech are also placed in ''marû''. In addition, different moods often require either a ''ḫamṭu'' or a ''marû'' stem and either a ''ḫamṭu'' or a ''marû'' agreement pattern depending on various conditions, as specified in the relevant sections #Modal prefixes, above and #Imperative, below. In more general terms, modern scholars usually state that the difference between the two forms is primarily one of grammatical aspect, aspect: ''ḫamṭu'' expresses perfective aspect, i.e. a completed action, or sometimes possibly Lexical aspect#Comrie's classification, punctual aspect, whereas ''marû'' expresses imperfective aspect, i.e. a non-completed action, or sometimes possibly durative aspect. In contrast, the ''time'' at which the action takes place or at which it is completed or non-completed is not specified and may be either past, present or future. This contrasts with the earlier view, prevalent in the first half of the 20th century, according to which the difference was one of grammatical tense, tense: ''ḫamṭu'' was thought to express the past tense, past (preterite) tense, and ''marû'' was considered to express present tense, present-future tense, while the use of ''marû'' with past-tense reference was viewed as a stylistic device (cf. the so-called historical present use in other languages). Indeed, it has been pointed out that a translation of ''ḫamṭu'' with past tense and ''marû'' with present or future tense does work well most of the time; this may correspond to the cases in which the action was viewed by Sumerian speakers as completed or non-completed ''with respect to the present moment''.The imperative mood
The imperative mood construction is produced with a ''ḫamṭu'' stem, but using the ''marû'' agreement pattern, by turning all prefixes into suffixes. In the plural, the second person plural ending is attached in a form that differs slightly from the indicative: it is /-(n)zen/, with the -/n/- appearing only after vowels. The stem is singular even in the plural imperative. Compare the following indicative-imperative pairs: This may be compared with the French pair ''vous le lui donnez'', but ''donnez-le-lui!'' In addition, the prefix 𒉌 ''i3-'' is replaced by /-a/: 𒉌𒁺 ''i3-g̃en'' "he went", but 𒁺𒈾 ''g̃en-na'' "go!", 𒅔𒈾𒀊𒁉 ''in-na-ab-be2'' "he will say it to him", but 𒅗𒂵𒀭𒈾(𒀊) ''dug4-ga-an-na(-ab)'' 'say it to him!'.Jagersma (2010: 556) However, the vowel /e/Foxvog (2016: 111-112) and possibly /i/ occasionally also occur if no further prefixes follow, perhaps as a characteristic of southern dialects. The ventive prefix ''mu-'', if not followed by others, has the form 𒌝 ''-um'' in the imperative: 𒁺𒌝 ''ře6-um'' 'bring it here!' In Old Babylonian texts, the reduced form -/u/ and the more regular -/am/ are also found: 𒂷𒉡 ''g̃e26-nu'', 𒁺𒀀𒀭 ''g̃en-am3'', both "come here!"Participles
Sumerian participles can function both as verbal adjectives and as verbal nouns. As verbal adjectives, they can describe any participant involved in the action or state expressed by the verb: for instance, 𒋧𒈠 ''šum2-ma'' may mean either "(which was) given (to someone)", "who was given (something)" or "who gave".Jagersma (2010: 628-629) As verbal nouns, they denote the action or state itself, so 𒋧𒈠 ''šum2-ma'' may also mean '(the act of) giving' or 'the fact that X gave Y'. Participles are formed in the following ways: * The bare ''ḫamṭu'' stem can function as a participle. It usually expresses timeless truths: 𒋧 ''šum2'' may be a person who regularly/constantly gives, something regularly given, or the regular act of giving. * Another way to form participles is by means of adding the nominalizing marker -/a/ to the ''ḫamṭu'' stem:Jagersma (2010: 627) 𒋧𒈠 ''šum2-ma'' "given". The verb form constructed in this way characterizes an entity with a specific action or state in the past or a state in the present (𒋾𒆷 ''til3-la'' "alive"). The verbs 𒌇 ''tuku'' "have" and 𒍪 ''zu'' "know" usually omit the ending -/a/, as does the verb 𒀝 ''ak'' "do". According to Jagersma, the nominalizing marker had the effect of geminating the preceding consonant (e.g. /šumːa/), which is evident from Akkadian loanwords, and this effect was due to its original form being /ʔa/ with a glottal stop that later assimilated to preceding consonants (/šumʔa/ > šumːa). * The ''marû'' stem can be combined with the suffix -/ed/ to form another participle, which often has a future and modal meaning similar to the gerundive#In Latin, Latin gerundive, e.g. 𒁶𒈨 ''dim2-me(-d)'' "which will/should be made". Adding a locative-terminative marker /-e/ after the /-ed/ yields a form with a meaning similar to the Latin conjugation#The gerund, Latin ''ad'' + gerund (acc.) construction: 𒁶(𒈨)𒉈 ''dim2(-me)-de3'' = "(in order) to make". A similar meaning can be expressed by adding the locative marker: 𒁶(𒈨)𒁕 ''dim2(-me)-da'' = "(for it) to be made". The main difference is that in the construction with ''-''(''ed'')-''e'', the subject of the intended action is the same as the subject of the main clause, while it is different in the construction with ''-''(''ed'')-''a''. The analysis of this participle is controversial along the same lines as that of the meaning of the suffix ''-ed'' in finite forms (see above). Some Sumerologists describe its meaning as primarily modal and distinguish it from a separate imperfective participle that consists of the ''marû'' stem alone, e.g. 𒁶𒈨 ''dim2-me'' 'which is/was making', 𒄄𒄄 ''gi4-gi4'' "returning". Others believe that it this is also the normal ''marû'' participle and that it has, in addition, the imperfective meanings "which is/was cutting" and "which is/was being cut". Besides the allomorphy of the suffix -/ed/ already treated above, the verb 𒅗 ''dug4'' "do, say" has a suppletive participial stem in this form: 𒁲 ''di(-d)''.Jagersma (2010: 656-660) * The ''marû'' stem can also occur with the suffix -/a/. Nonetheless, according to Jagersma, this form is rare outside the combination with a following possessive pronominal marker to express temporal meaning, as explained #Subordinate clauses, in the ''Syntax'' section: e.g. 𒁶(𒈨)𒁕𒉌 ''dim2(-me)-da-ni'' "when he makes (something)".Copula verb
The copula verb /me/ "to be" is mostly used in an enclitic form. Its conjugation is as follows: In addition, the initial vowel of the form ''-am3'' is reduced to -/m/ after enclitics ending in a vowel: 𒂍𒈬𒌝 ''e2''-''g̃u10''-''um'' "it is my house". Like other final consonants, the ''-m'' may not be expressed in early spelling. These enclitic forms are used instead of a simple sequence of finite prefix, root and personal suffix ''*i3-me-en'', ''*i-me'' etc. For more complex forms, the independent copula form is used: 𒉌𒈨𒀀 ''i3-me-a'' "that he is", 𒉡𒅇𒈨𒂗 ''nu-u3-me-en'' "I am not". Unlike the enclitic, it typically uses the normal stem 𒈨 -''me''- in the 3rd person singular (𒁀𒊏𒈨 ''ba-ra-me'' "should not be"), except for the form prefixed with ''ḫa-'', which is 𒃶𒅎 ''ḫe2-em'' or 𒃶𒀀𒀭 ''ḫe2-am3''.Jagersma (2010: 677-678) For a negative equivalent of the copula in the 3rd person, it seems that the word 𒉡 ''nu'' "not" alone instead of ''*nu-um'' is used predicatively (e.g. 𒍏𒉡 ''urud nu'' "it is not copper") although the form 𒉡(𒌦)𒂵𒀀𒀭 ''nu-(un)-ga-am3'' "it is also not ..." is attested. A different word is used to express existence or being present/located somewhere: ''𒅅 g̃al2''. A peculiar feature of the copula is that it seems to form a relative clause without the nominalizing suffix /-a/ and thus uses the finite form: thus, instead of 𒉌𒈨𒀀 ''i3-me-a'', simply 𒀀𒀭 -''am3'' is used: 𒆬𒃻𒂵𒊏𒉌𒅎𒈠𒀭𒋧 ''kug nig̃2-gur11-ra-ni-im ma-an-šum2'' "he gave me silver (which) was his property", which appears to say "The silver was his property, he gave it to me". In the negative, the full form 𒉡𒈨𒀀 ''nu-me-a'' "which is not" is used, and likewise in non-relative functions.Passive voice
Some scholars believe that it is possible to speak of a passive voice in Sumerian. Jagersma (2010) distinguishes three attested passive constructions. In each case, the ergative participant and the corresponding agreement marker on the verb are removed, so that the verb is inflected intransitively, but there may also be some additional cues to ensure a passive interpretation. The passive may be formed: # By simply eliminating the agent of a transitive verb and the corresponding agreement marker: "the farmer built the house" > † "the house was built". As a dynamic passive, in reference to the event itself, this construction is obsolete in ''ḫamṭu'' by the time of the earliest records according to Jagersma''.'' However, it is still used with modal prefixes and in ''marû'': e.g. "May the house be built!" Moreover, it continues to be used as a stative passive in Southern Sumerian, so can mean "the house is built (i.e. complete)". # With the prefix 𒁀 ''ba-'', e.g. . This is only found in Southern Sumerian and expresses only a dynamic passive, i.e. it refers to the event itself: "The house was (came to be) built". # With the prefix , e.g. . This is only found in Northern Sumerian and can have both a stative and a dynamic sense: "The house is built (complete)" or "The house was (came to be) built". The agent is never expressed in the passive clause in Sumerian. While the existence of such intransitive constructions of normally transitive verbs is widely recognized, some other scholars have disputed the view that these constructions should be called "passives". They prefer to speak of one-participant or agentless constructions and to limit themselves to the observation that the prefixes ''ba-'' and ''a-'' tend to be preferred with such constructions, apparently as a secondary effect of another, more subtle feature of their meaning. Concerning the history of the constructions, it has been claimed that the passive(-like) use of ''ba-'' does not appear before the Ur III period; Jagersma, on the contrary, states that it is attested already in the Old Sumerian period, although it becomes especially frequent in Ur III times. A different construction has been posited and labelled "Sumerian passive voice" by a significant number of scholars.Sallaberger (2023: 107); originally proposed by Claus Wilcke.Attinger (2009: 26-28) According to them, too, a passive is formed by removing the ergative participant and the verbal marker that agrees with it, but the verb is ''not'' inflected as an intransitive one: instead, it has a personal prefix, which refers to the "logical object": or "the house is being built". The stem is always ''ḫamṭu''. Some consider this construction to have only the function and meaning of a ''marû'' form'''', while others consider the tense-aspect opposition to be neutralized in it. The personal prefix is nearly always -''b''- in identified cases; views differ on whether it agrees in gender with an animate logical object, appearing as ''-n-'', or whether it remains ''-b-''. Critics have argued that most alleged examples of the construction are actually instances of #Expression of the directive by a pre-stem personal prefix, the pre-stem personal prefix referring to the directive participant in an intransitive verb, at least before the Old Babylonian period. Pascal Attinger considers it plausible that the original construction was indeed a directive one, whereas its new passive function as described by him arose via a reinterpretation in the Old Babylonian period; Walther Sallaberger, on the contrary, believes this kind of passive to be characteristic of Neo-Sumerian and to have been lost in Old Babylonian. A further possibility is that at least some of these cases actually have an Impersonal verb, impersonal 3rd person inanimate subject: "'it' has / they have built the house".Causative construction
Sumerian doesn't have dedicated causative morphology. Causative, Causativity is expressed syntactically in two ways, depending on the transitivity of the verb. # An intransitive verb is made transitive and thus acquires causative meaning merely by adding an ergative participant and the appropriate agreement marker: "the ox stood" - "the farmer made the ox stand". # A transitive verb is made causative by placing the ergative participant in the directive: "the farmer made the ox eat grass". For animates, as usual, the directive case marker is replaced by the dative one: "the farmer made the child eat bread". A further example can be : "the god made the farmer make the ox stand". # The causative constructions can in turn be passivized using the prefix ''ba-'': "the ox was caused to stand", "the ox was caused to eat grass" (), "the child was caused to eat bread". In Old Babylonian Sumerian, new causative markers have been claimed to have arisen under the influence of Akkadian; this is explained in the section on #Interference from Akkadian and other late phenomena, Interference from Akkadian and other late phenomena.Phrasal verbs
A specific problem of Sumerian syntax is posed by the numerous English phrasal verbs, phrasal verbs (traditionally called "compound verbs" in Sumerology in spite of the fact that they are not compounds, but idiomatic combinations). They usually involve a noun immediately before the verb, forming a lexical/idiomatic unit: e.g. 𒅆...𒂃 ''igi ...du8'', lit. "open the eye" = "see, look". Their case government and agreement patterns vary depending on the specific verb.Jagersma (2010: 300), Zólyomi (2017: 226-227) The component noun is usually in the absolutive case, but may be in the directive. If the phrasal verb takes another noun as a "logical object", the verbal infix is typically the directive, while the noun case is most commonly either the directive (dative if animate), which otherwise has the meaning "at / with respect to", or the locative (dative if animate), which otherwise has the meaning "on": * Directive: ** 𒅆...𒂃 ''igi ...du8'' (), lit. "open the eye at something" > "see"Zólyomi (2017: 218) ** 𒆥...𒀝 ''kig̃2 ...ak'', lit. "do work with respect to something" > "work (on) something" ** ''𒋗𒋳...𒅗 šu-tag ...dug4'', lit. "do hand-touching with respect to something" > "decorate" ** 𒊓...𒅗 ''sa2 dug4'', lit. "do equal with respect to something" > "reach" ** 𒄑...''𒋳 g̃eš ...tag'', lit. "make wood touch 'at' something" > "sacrifice something". ** 𒋛...𒁲 ''si ...sa2'' (), lit. "make the horns(?) equal with respect to something" > "put something in order"; likewise used intransitively: , lit. "the horns (?) are equal with respect to something" > "something is in order". * Locative "on": ** 𒅗...𒃻 ''inim'' ...''g̃ar'' (), lit. "place a word on something" > "claim, place a claim on" ** 𒋗...𒁇 ''šu ...bar'', lit. "open / remove the hand on something" > "release"Jagersma (2010: 440-441) ** ''𒈬...𒄷𒈿 mu ...sa4'', lit. "call a name on someone" > "to name" ** ''𒉆...𒋻 nam ...tar'', lit. "cut a fate upon someone" > "determine the fate of someone" ** ''𒀠...𒆕 al ...řu2'', lit. "raise the hoe upon something" > "dig" ** 𒇷...''𒋻 en3 ...tar'', lit. "cut a question(?) on something" > "investigate" Less commonly, the case of the logical object and the pronominal infix may be: * Dative (directive if inanimate): ** 𒆠...𒉘 ''ki'' ...''ag̃2'' () lit. "to measure out a place for someone" = "to love someone" ** ''𒅗 ...𒌣 gu3 ...de2'', lit. "to pour out the voice for someone" = "to call for someone" ** ''𒀀 ...𒊒 a ...ru'', lit. "to eject water for someone" = "to dedicate something to someone" * Terminative: 𒅆 ...𒁇 ''igi ...bar'' (NOUN-še igi ...bar) lit. "bring out the eye towards something" = "see, look" * Comitative: 𒀉 ...𒉘 ''a2'' ...''ag̃2'' () lit. "measure out power (?) with someone" = "to give orders to someone" * Locative "in": ** 𒋗... 𒁍 ''šu ...gid2'' (), lit. "stretch out the hand into something" = "to perform Haruspex, extispicy on" ** 𒋗... 𒁄 ''šu'' ...''bala'', lit. "let one's hand go across in something" = "alter" Another possibility is for the component noun to be in the dative (directive if inanimate), while the object is in the absolutive: * 𒋗...𒋾 ''šu ...ti'' () lit. "make something come close to the hand" = "to receive something" ("''from'' someone" is expressed by the terminative: )Syntax
General features
The basic word order is subject–object–verb; verb finality is only violated in rare instances, in poetry. The moving of a constituent towards the beginning of the phrase may be a way to highlight it,Zólyomi 1993 as may the addition of the copula to it. Grammatical modifier, Modifiers (adjectives, genitive phrases etc.) are normally placed after the noun: 𒂍𒉋 ''e2 gibil'' "a new house" 𒂍𒈗𒆷 ''e2 lugal-la'' "the house of the owner". However, the so-called anticipatory genitive (𒂍𒀀𒈗𒉈 ''e2-a lugal-bi'' "the owner of the house", lit. "of the house, its owner") is common and may signal the possessor's topic (linguistics), topicality. There are no adpositions, but noun phrases in a certain case may resemble prepositions and have a similar function:Thomsen (2001: 89) * 𒊮...𒀀𒅗 ''šag4 X-a-ka'', lit. "in the heart of X" = "inside/among X". * 𒅆 ... 𒀀𒂠 ''igi X-a-še3'', lit. "for the eyes of X" = "in front of X". * 𒂕...𒀀𒅗 ''egir X-a-ka'', lit. "at the back of X" = "behind/after X". * 𒀀𒅗...𒀀𒅗 X ''ugu2 X-a-ka'', lit. "on the skull of X" = "on top of X", "concerning X" * 𒁇...𒀀𒅗 ''bar X-a-ka'', lit. "outside of X" = "because of X" (in Old Sumerian). * 𒈬/𒉆 ... 𒀀𒂠 ''mu''/''nam X-a-še3'', lit. "for the name/fate of X" = "because of X" (in Neo-Sumerian).Subordinate clauses
There are various ways to express subordinate clause, subordination. Many of them include the nominalization of a finite verb with the suffix -/a/, which is also used to form participles, as shown above. Like the participles, this nominalized clause can either modify a noun, as adjectives do, or refer to the event itself, as nouns do. It usually functions as a relative clause, corresponding to an English clause with "which ..." or "who ...", as in the following example: Like the participles, the relative clauses can describe any participant involved in the action or state expressed by the verb, and the specific participant is determined by context: e.g. 𒈬𒌦𒈾𒀭𒋧𒈠 can be "which he gave to him", "who gave (something) to him", etc. The nominalized clause can also be a complement clause, corresponding to an English clause with "that ...", e.g. ''e2 in-řu2-a'' (''in-zu'') "(he knows) that he built the house". Like a noun, it can be followed by case morphemes: * In the locative case (with added 𒀀 ''-a''), it means "when": ''e2 in-řu2-a-a'' "when he built the house" (more literally "''in'' his building of the house"), although this is more common in Old Sumerian. * In the ablative case (with added 𒋫 ''-ta''), it means "after" or "since": ''e2 in-řu2-a-ta'' "after he built the house"; the particle 𒊑 ''-ri'' may express the same meaning as 𒋫 ''-ta''. * In the terminative case (with added 𒂠 -''še3''), it has a meaning close to "before" or "as to the fact that": ''e2 nu-řu2-a-še3'' "while he had not yet built the house". * In the equative case (with added 𒁶 -''gen7''), it can mean "as (if)", "as (when)", "when" or "because": ''e2 in-řu2-a-gen7'' "as he built the house". * It can also host the enclitics -/akanam/ and -/akeš/ "because": ''e2 in-řu2-a-ka-nam'' "because he built the house". * More surprisingly, it can add both the genitive and the locative morpheme with a meaning close to "when", possibly "as soon as": (''e2 in-řu2-a''-(''a-)ka'') "as soon as he built the house".Jagersma (2010: 594-626) The nominalized clause can directly modify a noun expressing time such as 𒌓 ''ud'' "day, time", 𒈬 ''mu'' "year" and 𒌗 ''itid'' "month", and this in turn can then stand in the locative and ablative in the same meanings as the clauses themselves: ''ud e2 in-řu2-a-a/ta'' "when/after he built the house". In this case, the particle ''-bi'' sometimes precedes the case morpheme: ''ud e2 in-řu2-a-ba''; the basic meaning is still of "when". The nominalized clause can also be included in the various "prepositional constructions" mentioned above: * ''bar e2 in-řu2-a-ka'' "because he built the house" (in Old Sumerian) * ''mu X-a-še3'' "because he built the house" (in Neo-Sumerian), * ''egir e2 in-řu2-a-ka'' "after he built the house". The structure is shown more clearly in the following example: Several clauses can be nominalized by a single enclitic: "he said that the fox had escaped and the farmer had not caught it". Participles can function in a very similar way to the nominalized clauses and be combined with the same kinds of adjuncts. One peculiarity is that, unlike nominalized clauses, they may also express the agent as a possessor, in the genitive case: 𒂍𒆕𒀀𒈗𒆷 ''e2 řu2-a lugal-la'' "the house built by the king". However, when the head noun (''e2'') is specified as here, a more common construction uses the ergative: 𒂍𒈗𒂊𒆕𒀀 ''e2 lugal-e řu2-a.'' A special subordinating construction with the temporal meaning of an English ''when''-clause is the so-called ''pronominal conjugation'', which contains a verb nominalized with -/a/ and following possessive pronominal markers referring to the subject (transitive or intransitive). In the 3rd person, the form appears to end in the possessive pronominal marker alone: 𒆭𒊏𒉌 ''kur9-ra-ni'' "when he entered", lit. "his entering", etc. It has been suggested that these forms actually also contain a final directive marker ''-e''; in this example, the analysis would be , "at his entering". Similarly, in Old Babylonian Sumerian, one sometimes finds the locative or ablative markers after the possessive (''kur9-ra-na, kur9-ra-ni-ta''). In contrast, in the 1st and 2nd persons, the 1st and 2nd person pronouns are followed by the syllable 𒉈 ''-ne'': 𒍣𒂵𒈬𒉈 ''zig3-ga-g̃u10-ne'' "as I rose"). The verb itself may be in ''ḫamṭu'', as in the above examples, or in ''marû'' followed by the modal/imperfective suffix -/ed/-: 𒍣𒍣𒁕𒈬𒉈 ''zi-zi-da''-''g̃u10-ne'' "when I rise". The same construction is used with the word 𒀸 ''dili'' "alone": 𒀸𒈬𒉈 ''dili-g̃u10-ne'' "I alone", etc. Subordinating conjunctions such as 𒌓𒁕 ''ud-da'' "when, if", 𒋗𒃻𒌉𒇲𒁉 ''tukum-bi'' "if" and 𒂗𒈾 ''en-na'' "until" also exist.Coordination
Coordinating conjunctions are rarely used. The most common way to express the sense of "and" is by simple juxtaposition. Nominal phrases may be conjoined, perhaps emphatically, by adding 𒁉 -''bi'' to the second one: 𒀭𒂗𒆤𒀭𒎏𒆤𒉌 ''en-lil2 nin-lil2-bi'' "both Enlil and Ninlil"; sometimes the enclitic is further reinforced by 𒁕 ''-da'' "with". More surprisingly, 𒋫 ''-ta'' "from" is also sometimes used in the sense of "and". The word 𒅇 ''u3'' "and" was borrowed from Akkadian in the Old Akkadian period and occurs mostly in relatively colloquial texts; Old Babylonian Sumerian also borrowed from Akkadian the enclitic 𒈠 ''-ma'' "and". There is no conjunction "or" and its sense can also be expressed by simple juxtaposition; a more explicit and emphatic alternative is the repetition of 𒃶𒅎 ''ḫe2-em'', "let it be": 𒇻𒃶𒅎𒈧𒃶𒅎 ''udu ḫe2-em maš ḫe2-em'' "(be it) a sheep or a goat"''.''Other issues
A quotative particle -/(e)še/ or -/ši/ "saying", variously spelt 𒂠 -''eše2'', 𒅆 -''ši'' or 𒀪𒊺 -''e-še'', has been identified. Its use is not obligatory and it is attested only or almost only in texts from the Old Babylonian period or later. Another, rarely attested, particle, 𒄑(𒊺)𒂗 -''g̃eš(-še)-en'', apparently expresses irrealis modality: "were it that ...". Highlighting uses of the copula somewhat similar to English Cleft sentence, cleft constructions are present: 𒈗𒀀𒀭𒉌𒁺 ''lugal-am3 i3-g̃en'' "It is the king who came", 𒀀𒈾𒀸𒀀𒀭𒉌𒁺 ''a-na-aš''-''am3 i3-g̃en'' "Why is it that he came?", 𒉌𒁺𒈾𒀀𒀭 ''i3-g̃en'' "It is the case that he came". Sumerian generally links a nominal predicate to the subject using the copula verb, like English. However, it does use Zero copula, zero-copula constructions in some contexts. In interrogative sentences, the 3rd person copula is omitted: 𒀀𒈾𒈬𒍪 ''a-na mu-zu'' "What is your name?", 𒉈𒂗𒈬𒍪 ''ne-en mu-zu'' "Is this your name?". Sumerian proper names that consist of entire sentences normally lack a copula as well, e.g. 𒀀𒁀𒀭𒌓𒁶 ''a-ba dutu-gen7'' "Who is like Shamash, Utu?" As explained #Copula verb, above, negative sentences also omit the copula in *''nu-am3''/''nu-um "''isn't" and use simply 𒉡 ''nu'' instead. ''Yes/no''-interrogative sentences appear to have been marked only by intonation and possibly by resulting lengthening of final vowels. There is no wh-movement to the beginning of the clause, but the interrogative words are placed immediately before the verb: e.g. 𒈗𒂊𒀀𒈾𒈬𒌦𒀝 ''lugal-e a-na mu-un-ak'' "What did the king do?", 𒂍𒀀𒁀𒀀𒅔𒆕 ''e2 a-ba-a in-řu3'' "Who built the temple?" Two exceptions from this are that the constituent noun of a #Phrasal verbs, phrasal verb is normally closer to the verb,Jagersma (2010: 228) and that an interrogative word emphasized with a copula such as 𒀀𒈾𒀸𒀀𒀭 ''a-na-aš-am3'' "why is it that ...?" is placed at the beginning of the clause. In addition, as already mentioned, interrogative sentences omit the copula where a declarative would have used it.Word formation
Derivation by affixation is largely non-existent. An exception may be a few nouns ending in -/u/ denoting the object of a corresponding verb: 𒊬𒊒 ''sar-ru'' "document" < 𒊬 ''sar'' "write". Compounding, on the other hand, is common in nouns. Compounds are normally left-headed. The dependent may be: * Another noun: 𒂍 ''e2'' "house" + 𒈬 ''muḫaldim'' "cook" > 𒂍𒈬 ''e2''-''muḫaldim'' "kitchen" * An adjective: 𒌨 ''ur'' "dog" + 𒈤''maḫ'' "great" > 𒌨𒈤 ''ur-maḫ'' "lion" * A participle (consisting of the bare verb stem): 𒃻 ''nig̃2'' "thing" + 𒁀 ''ba'' "give(n)" > 𒃻𒁀 ''nig̃2-ba'' "present", * A participle with a dependent word: 𒃻 ''nig̃2'' "thing" ''+ 𒍣 zi'' "breath" ''+ 𒅅 g̃al2'' "be there" > 𒃻𒍣𒅅 ''nig̃2-zi-g̃al2'' "living thing" An older obsolete pattern was right-headed instead: * ''𒂍 e2'' "house" ''+ 𒊮 šag4'' "heart" ''> 𒂍𒊮 e2''-''šag4'' "innermost part of a house" * 𒃲 ''gal'' "big" + 𒈜 ''nar'' "musician" > 𒃲𒈜 ''gal-nar'' "chief musician" A participle may be the head of the compound, preceded by a dependent: * 𒁾 ''dub'' "clay tablet" + ''𒊬 sar'' "write" > 𒁾''𒊬 dub-sar'' "scribe" * 𒋗 ''šu'' "hand" + 𒋳 ''tag'' "touch" > 𒋗𒋳 ''šu-tag'' "decoration" (corresponding to the phrasal verb 𒋗...𒋳 ''šu...tag'' "decorate") There are a few cases of nominalized finite verbs, too: 𒁀𒍗 ''ba-uš4'' "(who) has died" > "dead" Abstract nouns are formed as compounds headed by the word 𒉆 ''nam-'' "fate, status": 𒌉 ''dumu'' "child" > 𒉆𒌉 ''nam-dumu'' "childhood", 𒋻 ''tar'' "cut, decide" > 𒉆𒋻 ''nam-tar'' "fate". Nouns that express the object of an action or an object possessing a characteristic are formed as compounds headed by the word 𒃻 ''nig̃2'' "thing": 𒅥 ''gu4'' "eat" > 𒃻𒅥 ''nig̃2''-''gu7'' "food", 𒄭 "good, sweet" > 𒃻𒄭 ''nig̃2-dug'' "something sweet". The meaning may also be abstract: 𒋛...𒁲 ''si...sa2'' "straighten, put in order" > ''nig̃2-si-sa2'' "justice". A small number of terms of professions are derived with the preposed element 𒉡 ''nu-'': 𒄑𒊬 ''g̃eškiri6'' "garden" ''> 𒉡𒄑𒊬 nu-g̃eškiri6-(k)'' "gardener". Apparent coordinative compounds also exist, e.g. 𒀭𒆠 ''an-ki'' "the universe", lit. "heaven and earth". A noun can be formed from an adjective by conversion: for example, 𒂼 ''dag̃al'' "wide" also means "width". On verbs acquiring the properties of adjectives and nouns (agent nouns and action nouns), see the section on ''#Participles, Participles''. While new verbs cannot be derived, verbal meanings may be expressed by phrasal verbs (see above); in particular, new phrasal verbs are often formed on the basis of nouns by making them the object of the verbs 𒅗 ''dug4'' "do" or 𒀝 ''ak'' "make": 𒀀...𒅗 ''a ...dug4'', lit. "to do water" > "to irrigate", 𒄑𒂵...𒍮 ''g̃ešga-rig2 ...ak'', lit. "to do the comb" > "to comb".Dialects
The standard variety of Sumerian was (: ). A notable variety or sociolect was (: ), possibly to be interpreted as "fine tongue" or "high-pitched voice". Other apparent terms for Register (sociolinguistics), registers or dialects were ''eme-galam'' "high tongue", ''eme-si-sa2'' "straight tongue", ''eme-te-na2'' "oblique[?] tongue", ''emesukudda'', ''emesuha'', ''emesidi'' and ''emeku''. Recently, a regional differentiation into a Northern and a Southern Sumerian dialect area has been posited.Emesal
''Emesal'' is used exclusively by female characters in some literary texts. In addition, it is dominant in certain genres of cult songs such as the hymns sung by Gala (priests), Gala priests. It has been argued that it might have been a female language variety of the kind that exists or has existed in some cultures, such as among the Chukchi language, Chukchis and the Garifuna language#Gender differences, Garifuna. Alternatively, it has been contended that it must have been originally a regional dialect, since instances of apparent Emesal-like forms are attested in the area of late 3rd millennium Lagash, and some loanwords into Akkadian appear to come from Emesal rather than Emegir. Apart from such isolated glosses, Emesal is first attested in writing in the early Old Babylonian period. It is typically written with syllable signs rather than logograms. A text is often not written consistently in Emesal, but contains apparent Emegir forms as well. The special features of ''Emesal'' are mostly phonological and lexical. In terms of phonology, the following are some of the most common sound correspondences: There are also specifically Emesal lexemes that do not seem to be cognate with their Emegir counterparts, for example: In grammar, both the cohortative prefix 𒂵 ''ga-'' and the precative prefix 𒄩 ''ḫa-'' are replaced by the morpheme 𒁕 ''da-'' (with the allomorphs 𒉈 ''de3''- and 𒂅 ''du5''- conditioned by context in the same way as that of the corresponding Emegir prefixes).Southern and Northern Sumerian
Bram Jagersma and Gábor ZólyomiZólyomi (2017: 19) distinguish two regional dialects of Sumerian: the Southern Sumerian dialect ofOld Babylonian Sumerian
The dominant Sumerian variety of the Old Babylonian period, in turn, reflected a different regional dialect from the standard Neo-Sumerian of the Ur III period: * Neo-Sumerian elides the conjugation prefixes 𒉌 /i/- and 𒀀 /a/- in front of the prefixes 𒉌-/ni/-, 𒊏 -/ra/- and 𒊑 -/ri/-, while Old Babylonian Sumerian retains them. * The original sequence 𒈬𒂊 ''mu-e-'', consisting of the ventive conjugation prefix 𒈬 ''mu-'' and the 2nd person prefix 𒂊 ''-e-'', is contracted into 𒈬 /muː/ in the Ur III standard, but into 𒈨 /meː/ in the most common Old Babylonian variety. * In general, Old Babylonian Sumerian preserved many features of Northern Sumerian, in contrast to the decidedly Southern character of the Ur III standard. This is doubtlessly connected to the fact that the centre of power in Babylonia moved to the north. In particular, it uses spellings that show that its reflex of the Old Sumerian ''ř'' phoneme is /d/.Interference from Akkadian and other late phenomena
In the Old Babylonian period and after it, the Sumerian used by scribes was influenced by their mother tongue, Akkadian, and sometimes more generally by imperfect acquisition of the language. As a result, various deviations from its original structure occur in texts or copies of texts from these times. The following effects have been found in the Old Babylonian period: * confusion of the animate and inanimate gender, resulting in use of incorrect gender pronouns; * occasional use of the animate plural ''-ene'' with inanimates; * occasional use of the directive case marker -/e/ with animates; * changes in the use of the nominal case markers so as to parallel the use of Akkadian prepositions, whereas the verbal case markers remain unchanged, resulting in mismatches between nominal and verbal case; * generalized use of terminative -/še/ to express direction, displacing locative -/a/ as the expression of Illative case, illative and Sublative case, sublative meanings ("into" and "onto") and directive -/e/ as the expression of achieving contiguity with something; * treatment of the prefix sequences /b/-/i/- and /n/-/i/-, which originally could mark the causee in transitive verbs, as causative markers even with intransitive verbs; * dropping of final -/m/ in the copula -/am/ and sometimes its replacement with -/e/; * occurrence of -/e/ as a ''marû'' 3rd person singular marker even in intransitive verbs;ETCSRI's Morphological Parsing. Accessed 13.06.2024Syllabary
The table below shows signs used for simple syllables of the form CV or VC. As used for the Sumerian language, the cuneiform script was in principle capable of distinguishing at least 16 consonants, (about phonemes g̃ and ř and their representation using cuneiform signs). transliterated as as well as four vowel qualities, ''a, e, i, u''.Sample text
Inscription by Entemena of Lagaš
This text was inscribed on a small clay cone . It recounts the beginning of a war between the city-states of Lagaš and Umma during the Early Dynastic III period, one of the earliest border conflicts recorded. (RIME 1.09.05.01)See also
*List of languages by first written accounts * Pennsylvania Sumerian Dictionary *Sumerian literature *Descent of Inanna into the UnderworldReferences
Notes
Citations
Bibliography
Further reading
*Cohen, Mark E., "An Annotated Sumerian Dictionary", University Park, USA: Penn State University Press, 2023 * *Ebeling, J., & Cunningham, G. (2007). ''Analysing literary Sumerian : corpus-based approaches''. London: Equinox. *Debourse, Céline and Gabbay, Uri, "The Late Babylonian Series of ‘Ancient Sumerian’: Structure, Contents, and the Agency of Ritual Texts", Zeitschrift für Assyriologie und vorderasiatische Archäologie, vol. 114, no. 1, pp. 28-42, 2024External links
*''General''