連濁
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

is a pronunciation change seen in some
compound words In linguistics, a compound is a lexeme (less precisely, a word or sign) that consists of more than one stem. Compounding, composition or nominal composition is the process of word formation that creates compound lexemes. Compounding occurs when t ...
in
Japanese Japanese may refer to: * Something from or related to Japan, an island country in East Asia * Japanese language, spoken mainly in Japan * Japanese people, the ethnic group that identifies with Japan through ancestry or culture ** Japanese diaspor ...
. When rendaku occurs, a
voiceless In linguistics, voicelessness is the property of sounds being pronounced without the larynx vibrating. Phonologically, it is a type of phonation, which contrasts with other states of the larynx, but some object that the word phonation implies v ...
consonant (such as ) is replaced with a voiced consonant (such as ) at the start of the second (or later) part of the compound. For example, the
morpheme A morpheme is any of the smallest meaningful constituents within a linguistic expression and particularly within a word. Many words are themselves standalone morphemes, while other words contain multiple morphemes; in linguistic terminology, this ...
starts with the voiceless consonant when used as an independent word or as the first part of a compound word, but this is replaced with the corresponding voiced consonant in the compound word , from + . Although rendaku is common, it does not occur in all compound words. A rule known as Lyman's law blocks rendaku when the second element already contains a voiced obstruent phoneme (, , , or ). For instance, in , the in remains voiceless because contains . Rendaku is also blocked almost always when the second element of a compound is a recent loan into Japanese. Furthermore, rendaku may fail to occur even in contexts where no definite blocking factor is present. In the
Japanese writing system The modern Japanese writing system uses a combination of Logogram, logographic kanji, which are adopted Chinese characters, and Syllabary, syllabic kana. Kana itself consists of a pair of syllabary, syllabaries: hiragana, used primarily for n ...
, rendaku affects how a morpheme is spelled when using one of the kana syllabaries: it causes the ''
dakuten The , colloquially , is a diacritic most often used in the Japanese kana syllabaries to indicate that the consonant of a mora should be pronounced voiced, for instance, on sounds that have undergone rendaku (sequential voicing). The , coll ...
'' ("voicing mark", written as ゛) to be added to the upper right corner of the kana character that represents the first consonant and vowel in the second element of the compound. This is seen when comparing the
hiragana is a Japanese language, Japanese syllabary, part of the Japanese writing system, along with ''katakana'' as well as ''kanji''. It is a phonetic lettering system. The word ''hiragana'' means "common" or "plain" kana (originally also "easy", ...
spelling of () to that of (). Rendaku is not marked in writing when a morpheme is spelled using
kanji are logographic Chinese characters, adapted from Chinese family of scripts, Chinese script, used in the writing of Japanese language, Japanese. They were made a major part of the Japanese writing system during the time of Old Japanese and are ...
(
logograph In a written language, a logogram (from Ancient Greek 'word', and 'that which is drawn or written'), also logograph or lexigraph, is a written character that represents a semantic component of a language, such as a word or morpheme. Chines ...
s taken from
Chinese characters Chinese characters are logographs used Written Chinese, to write the Chinese languages and others from regions historically influenced by Chinese culture. Of the four independently invented writing systems accepted by scholars, they represe ...
). For example, is written with the kanji character , which is unchanged when used in the spelling of . Linguistically, rendaku involves aspects of both pronunciation (
phonology Phonology (formerly also phonemics or phonematics: "phonemics ''n.'' 'obsolescent''1. Any procedure for identifying the phonemes of a language from a corpus of data. 2. (formerly also phonematics) A former synonym for phonology, often pre ...
) and word structure (
morphology Morphology, from the Greek and meaning "study of shape", may refer to: Disciplines *Morphology (archaeology), study of the shapes or forms of artifacts *Morphology (astronomy), study of the shape of astronomical objects such as nebulae, galaxies, ...
), and so is categorized as a
morphophonological Morphophonology (also morphophonemics or morphonology) is the branch of linguistics that studies the interaction between morphological and phonological or phonetic processes. Its chief focus is the sound changes that take place in morphemes (m ...
phenomenon.


Effects

Rendaku replaces a voiceless
obstruent An obstruent ( ) is a speech sound such as , , or that is formed by ''obstructing'' airflow. Obstruents contrast with sonorants, which have no such obstruction and so resonate. All obstruents are consonants, but sonorants include vowels as well ...
consonant with a voiced consonant sound. For example, the
voiceless alveolar plosive The voiceless alveolar, dental and postalveolar plosives (or stops) are types of consonantal sounds used in almost all spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents voiceless dental, alveolar, and postal ...
becomes the
voiced alveolar plosive The voiced alveolar, dental and postalveolar plosives (or stops) are types of consonantal sounds used in many spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents voiced dental, alveolar, and postalveolar plosi ...
in the context of rendaku. In this case, the new consonant retains the same manner and
place of articulation In articulatory phonetics, the place of articulation (also point of articulation) of a consonant is an approximate location along the vocal tract where its production occurs. It is a point where a constriction is made between an active and a pa ...
as the original consonant. However, rendaku can also cause additional changes depending on the sounds involved.


Pronunciation

In the context of
Japanese phonology Japanese phonology is the system of sounds used in the pronunciation of the Japanese language. Unless otherwise noted, this article describes the standard variety of Japanese based on the Tokyo dialect. There is no overall consensus on the nu ...
, some consonant sounds that seem distinct to English speakers, and that have distinct spellings in
Hepburn romanization is the main system of Romanization of Japanese, romanization for the Japanese language. The system was originally published in 1867 by American Christian missionary and physician James Curtis Hepburn as the standard in the first edition of h ...
, are analyzed as
allophones In phonology, an allophone (; from the Greek , , 'other' and , , 'voice, sound') is one of multiple possible spoken soundsor '' phones''used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language. For example, in English, the voiceless plosi ...
(contextual variants of a
phoneme A phoneme () is any set of similar Phone (phonetics), speech sounds that are perceptually regarded by the speakers of a language as a single basic sound—a smallest possible Phonetics, phonetic unit—that helps distinguish one word fr ...
). The following table describes the effects of rendaku in the standard variety of Japanese, using both phonemic transcriptions (marked by slashes, such as ) and phonetic transcriptions (marked by square brackets, such as ). Depending on the accent and age of a speaker, the
voiced velar nasal The voiced velar nasal, also known as eng, engma, or agma (from Greek 'fragment'), is a type of consonantal sound used in some spoken languages. It is the sound of ''ng'' in English ''sing'' as well as ''n'' before velar consonants as in ''E ...
may be used in Japanese as an alternative to the
voiced velar stop The voiced velar plosive or stop is a type of consonantal sound used in many spoken languages. Some languages have the voiced pre-velar plosive, which is articulated slightly more front compared with the place of articulation of the prototypic ...
, mainly in cases where the consonant occurs in the middle of a word. For speakers who use in the middle of words, is replaced with in the context of rendaku. The sounds and are typically analyzed as allophones of a single phoneme (although some phonologists have argued they are distinct phonemes for the minority of speakers who consistently distinguish them). The voiceless
affricate An affricate is a consonant that begins as a stop and releases as a fricative, generally with the same place of articulation (most often coronal). It is often difficult to decide if a stop and fricative form a single phoneme or a consonant pai ...
s are commonly analyzed as allophones of . Originally, corresponded to voiced affricate sounds , whereas corresponded to voiced fricative sounds . However, the historical distinction between and has been lost in the Tokyo-based standard (though not in all regional varieties of Japanese). The merged sounds may be pronounced either as voiced affricates or voiced fricatives, regardless of etymology (see ''
yotsugana are a set of four specific kana, じ, ぢ, ず, づ (in the Nihon-shiki romanization system: ''zi'', ''di'', ''zu'', ''du''), used in the Japanese writing system. They historically represented four distinct voiced morae (syllables) in ...
''). Therefore, historical and have merged as phonetic , and historical and have merged as phonetic . In the context of rendaku, dialects with the merger may be analyzed as possessing an underlying phonemic distinction between and that becomes neutralized on the phonetic level as . Alternatively, treats rendaku as replacing the phoneme with before . A third approach is taken by , who rejects the identification of as allophones of , and instead postulates five voiceless phonemes and three voiced phonemes , with rendaku turning into , into , and into , respectively. Rendaku turns the
voiceless glottal fricative The voiceless glottal fricative, sometimes called voiceless glottal transition or the aspirate, is a type of sound used in some spoken languages that patterns like a fricative or approximant consonant '' phonologically'', but often lacks the ...
into the
voiced bilabial plosive The voiced bilabial plosive or stop is a type of consonantal sound used in many spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is , and the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is b. The voiced bilabial stop o ...
. This is because Japanese descends originally from a
voiceless bilabial plosive The voiceless bilabial plosive or stop is a type of consonantal sound used in most Speech communication, spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is , and the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is p. F ...
*. Before the end of the 16th century, * developed into a bilabial fricative or labiodental fricative . Then, during the 17-18th centuries, this or developed into glottal before and palatal before or , remaining only before the vowel phoneme (which is currently pronounced ). As a result, in modern Japanese, rendaku replaces with .


Spelling

It is possible to characterize rendaku in terms of its effect on a morpheme's Japanese
kana are syllabary, syllabaries used to write Japanese phonology, Japanese phonological units, Mora (linguistics), morae. In current usage, ''kana'' most commonly refers to ''hiragana'' and ''katakana''. It can also refer to their ancestor , wh ...
spelling: it adds the ''
dakuten The , colloquially , is a diacritic most often used in the Japanese kana syllabaries to indicate that the consonant of a mora should be pronounced voiced, for instance, on sounds that have undergone rendaku (sequential voicing). The , coll ...
'' (voicing mark) to the first kana of the affected morpheme. The relevant
graphemes In linguistics, a grapheme is the smallest functional unit of a writing system. The word ''grapheme'' is derived from Ancient Greek ('write'), and the suffix ''-eme'' by analogy with ''phoneme'' and other emic units. The study of graphemes ...
are shown in the tables below (excluding
yōon The is a feature of the Japanese language in which a mora is formed with an added sound, i.e., palatalized, or (more rarely in the modern language) with an added sound, i.e. labialized. ''Yōon'' are represented in hiragana using a kana end ...
digraphs, which are formed by taking an i-column kana and placing a small ''ya, yu,'' or ''yo'' kana after it). When morphemes that begin with the morae ''chi'' (/) and ''tsu'' (/) undergo rendaku, the resulting morae ''ji'' and ''zu'' are generally spelled with the kana / and /, rather than the identically pronounced / and /. This is not a strict rule, however, and is relaxed in certain older compounds or names, especially those that are not easily recognized as compounds.


Examples

The following table shows an example of rendaku for each major allophone of the eligible consonant sounds:


Historical origin

The voiced obstruent consonants of modern Japanese come from the
prenasalized Prenasalized consonants are phonetic sequences of a nasal and an obstruent (or occasionally a non-nasal sonorant) that behave phonologically like single consonants. The primary reason for considering them to be single consonants, rather than clus ...
obstruents of
Old Japanese is the oldest attested stage of the Japanese language, recorded in documents from the Nara period (8th century). It became Early Middle Japanese in the succeeding Heian period, but the precise delimitation of the stages is controversial. Old Ja ...
. Rendaku may have originated from the fusion of consonants with preceding nasal sounds derived from reduction of either the genitive postposition or the dative postposition . For example, may be derived from "mountain- bird", by means of fusing with the following to form prenasalized in Old Japanese. This hypothesis helps explain why rendaku is not found in all compound words. If some compounds were originally formed with or , but others were formed through simple juxtaposition of roots, then rendaku would develop regularly only in the first category. By the Old Japanese period, rendaku had already become grammatically distinct from constructions with or . This is shown by the use of forms such as "your bird": a corresponding phrase with would be ungrammatical, since the genitive of the pronoun "you" was formed exclusively with the postposition .


Conditions

Rendaku occurs most frequently in compounds where the second element is a single morpheme of native Japanese origin that does not already contain a voiced obstruent phoneme. However, even though rendaku is usual in this context, it does not invariably occur: there are numerous exceptions pronounced without rendaku. Some compound words are pronounced with rendaku by some speakers, but without it by other speakers. Rendaku is blocked when a voiced obstruent phoneme is already present in the second element of the compound. This rule, called Lyman’s law, is highly reliable, with only a small number of exceptions. Rendaku is also blocked if the second element is a recent loanword from a language other than Chinese (
gairaigo is Japanese for "loan word", and indicates a transcription into Japanese. In particular, the word usually refers to a Japanese word of foreign origin that was not borrowed in ancient times from Old or Middle Chinese (especially Literary Chine ...
). This rule likewise has few exceptions. Rendaku does not affect most Sino-Japanese elements, but this tendency is less consistent. A substantial minority do undergo rendaku as the second element of a compound. It has been speculated that Sino-Japanese elements that can undergo rendaku might have become "vulgarized", that is, adopted into the same category of vocabulary as native Japanese morphemes. Other rules have been proposed regarding circumstances where rendaku is either blocked or favored, but often, it is only possible to identify tendencies rather than inviolable rules.


Lyman's law

Lyman's Law is a fundamental constraint on rendaku that prohibits voicing when the second element of a compound already contains a voiced obstruent phoneme (, , , or , including
allophone In phonology, an allophone (; from the Greek , , 'other' and , , 'voice, sound') is one of multiple possible spoken soundsor '' phones''used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language. For example, in English, the voiceless plos ...
s such as and ). These phonemes are all written with the
dakuten The , colloquially , is a diacritic most often used in the Japanese kana syllabaries to indicate that the consonant of a mora should be pronounced voiced, for instance, on sounds that have undergone rendaku (sequential voicing). The , coll ...
and called in Japanese. For example: * + → (surname) ("mountain" + "gate" → place name). Rendaku does not occur because already contains a voiced obstruent consonant phoneme . Therefore, its initial consonant remains voiceless, and the form *''Yamagado'' やまがど does not exist (* indicates a non-existent form). * + → ("one person" + "travel" → "traveling alone"), not *. Rendaku does not occur because already contains a voiced obstruent consonant phoneme . * + → ("mountain" + "fire" → "mountain fire"), not *. Rendaku does not occur because already contains a voiced obstruent consonant phoneme (in the form of its allophone ). The precise formulation of Lyman's law varies between analysts. In particular, there are conflicting viewpoints on the size of the phonological domain in which it applies. One formulation states that ''rendaku'' is blocked by the presence of voiced obstruent consonant "within a morpheme"; this may be interpreted as a consequence of a more general constraint that applies also to the underived form of native Japanese morphemes, which do not generally contain more than one voiced obstruent phoneme. An alternative view is that Lyman's law applies whenever a voiced obstruent consonant occurs anywhere within the second element of a compound, and so might also include cases where this element is composed of more than one morpheme. Rendaku occasionally (although relatively infrequently) causes voicing of the initial consonant of a Sino-Japanese () lexeme that is written with two kanji, and is in etymological terms composed of two Sino-Japanese roots; it is debatable whether such lexemes count as one morpheme or two from a
synchronic Synchronic may refer to: * ''Synchronic'' (film), a 2019 American science fiction film starring Anthony Mackie and Jamie Dornan *Synchronic analysis, the analysis of a language at a specific point of time *Synchronicity, the experience of two or m ...
perspective. In modern Japanese, the presence of a voiced obstruent phoneme in the first element does not normally block rendaku, as demonstrated by examples such as + → . Nevertheless, it has been proposed that in certain circumstances, Lyman's law might be (or might once have been) sensitive to the presence of a voiced obstruent in the first element of a compound. Compound personal names ending in the element 'rice field' seem to usually show rendaku when the final mora of the first element contains (e.g. ), but never show rendaku when the final mora of the first element contains (e.g. ) (and usually do not show rendaku when it contains ). The pattern of voicing seen in compounds like these may in part be a residue of an older version of the law that operated in Old Japanese. Examination of Old Japanese compounds suggests that Old Japanese had a constraint against two consecutive syllables starting with a
prenasalized Prenasalized consonants are phonetic sequences of a nasal and an obstruent (or occasionally a non-nasal sonorant) that behave phonologically like single consonants. The primary reason for considering them to be single consonants, rather than clus ...
consonant (the source of modern Japanese /voiced obstruent phonemes), but over time, this constraint came to be replaced with the modern tendency for a consonant to block rendaku only when it occurs in the second element of the compound. Some formulations of the law treat it as applicable only in cases where the /voiced obstruent phoneme is the second consonant of the final element of the compound. In the case of Old Japanese, there is not enough evidence to conclude whether the Old Japanese version of Lyman's law applied to morphemes containing a prenasalized consonant in their third or later syllable. In modern Japanese, however, there is evidence that Lyman's law applies in general to morphemes of this form, as indicated by the lack of rendaku in examples such as + → or + → . There are only a handful of exceptions, such as + → , where voicing occurs despite the presence of a /voiced obstruent consonant in the second element of the compound. Although this law is named after
Benjamin Smith Lyman Benjamin Smith Lyman (11 December 1835 – 30 August 1920) was an American mining engineer, surveyor, and an amateur linguist and anthropologist. He was also a promoter of vegetarianism. Biography Benjamin Smith Lyman was born in Northampton, Ma ...
, who independently propounded it in 1894, it is really a re-discovery. The
Edo period The , also known as the , is the period between 1600 or 1603 and 1868 in the history of Japan, when the country was under the rule of the Tokugawa shogunate and some 300 regional ''daimyo'', or feudal lords. Emerging from the chaos of the Sengok ...
linguists Kamo no Mabuchi (1765) and
Motoori Norinaga was a Japanese people, Japanese scholar of active during the Edo period. He is conventionally ranked as one of the Four Great Men of Kokugaku (nativist) studies. Life Norinaga was born in what is now Matsusaka, Mie, Matsusaka in Ise Province ...
(1767–1798) separately and independently identified the law during the 18th century.


Source language of the second element

Another important factor affecting the likelihood of rendaku is the etymological source or lexical stratum of the second element of the compound. Rendaku frequently affects
wago are native Japanese words, meaning those words in Japanese that have been inherited from Old Japanese, rather than being borrowed at some stage. Together with kango () and gairaigo (), they form one of the three main sources of Japanese words ...
(native Japanese lexemes), infrequently affects
kango Kango is a town in the Estuaire Province of Gabon, Central Africa, lying on the Komo River and the N1 road. It has a station near the Trans-Gabon Railway, where the railway bridges the Gabon Estuary. Kango is a small town with a population ...
(Sino-Japanese vocabulary), and very rarely affects
gairaigo is Japanese for "loan word", and indicates a transcription into Japanese. In particular, the word usually refers to a Japanese word of foreign origin that was not borrowed in ancient times from Old or Middle Chinese (especially Literary Chine ...
(recent loanwords, such as borrowings from English). On the other hand, the lexical stratum of the first element of the compound is not relevant. One possible reason for the resistance of Sino-Japanese morphemes to rendaku is the greater potential for it to cause homophony in this context. Native Japanese morphemes very rarely start with a voiced obstruent consonant, but this does not apply to Sino-Japanese; therefore, rendaku of Sino-Japanese morphemes is more likely to neutralize a contrast between distinct morphemes.


Kango

Sino-Japanese vocabulary is built from Sino-Japanese roots, which have a restricted phonological structure (one or two moras long). At least half of Sino-Japanese terms are "binoms" consisting of two roots, but some roots can be used on their own as words (mononoms). A binom is written with two
kanji are logographic Chinese characters, adapted from Chinese family of scripts, Chinese script, used in the writing of Japanese language, Japanese. They were made a major part of the Japanese writing system during the time of Old Japanese and are ...
(Chinese characters), one for each root. Most Sino-Japanese lexemes do not undergo rendaku when used as the second element of a compound. However, a minority (around 20% of mononoms, and 10% of binoms) do show rendaku in at least some compounds. No criteria have been identified that predict with 100% accuracy when this occurs. In terms of phonology, rendaku is blocked by Lyman's law and so never affects a binom where the second root starts with a voiced obstruent phoneme; e.g. in , in . Based on a study of how native speakers pronounced novel compounds, concluded that rendaku is statistically less likely to affect a Sino-Japanese binom where the first root ends in the moraic nasal and the second root end in a voiceless obstruent, although it is not fully blocked in this context (the study's observed rate of rendaku in this context was 1.3%, compared to Vance 1996's dictionary-based estimate of rendaku affecting 10% of all Sino-Japanese binoms). Sino-Japanese roots that start with voiceless obstruents may have variant pronunciations starting with voiced obstruent phonemes for other reasons aside from rendaku. One reason is the existence of different readings of Sino-Japanese roots, corresponding in general to different time periods of borrowing. Two of the most important types of reading are termed Go-on and Kan-on. Some (though not all) Kan-on readings starting with correspond regularly to Go-on readings starting with : these represent different adaptations of Early Middle Chinese voiced obstruent sounds, and so the existence of these alternative pronunciations is unrelated in origin to rendaku. An example is the root 'ground, land', which has both a Kan-on pronunciation and a Go-on pronunciation . For this reason, the use of the voiced pronunciation in compounds such as cannot necessarily be attributed to rendaku, since the version of this root can also be found at the start of a word, e.g. in . In addition, a minority of lexemes composed of two Sino-Japanese roots display a type of sequential voicing, affecting only roots in second position, that is indistinguishable in effect from rendaku, but probably has a distinct origin in terms of morphology. The relevant context is forms such as , which appears to display rendaku on its second element . In this case, voicing is hypothesized to be the result of the preceding Sino-Japanese root originally ending in a nasal sound, which caused the following consonant to become prenasalized and voiced. Prenasalization and voicing of a consonant after a nasal sound () is hypothesized to have been an active phonological rule up through
Early Middle Japanese is a stage of the Japanese language between 794 and 1185, which is known as the Heian period (). The successor to Old Japanese (), it is also known as Late Old Japanese. However, the term "Early Middle Japanese" is preferred, as it is closer to ...
; in
Late Middle Japanese was a stage of the Japanese language following Early Middle Japanese and preceding Early Modern Japanese. It was a period of transition in which the language shed many of its archaic features and became closer to its modern form. The period ...
, were denasalized and voicing after ceased to be automatic.


Gairaigo

''
Gairaigo is Japanese for "loan word", and indicates a transcription into Japanese. In particular, the word usually refers to a Japanese word of foreign origin that was not borrowed in ancient times from Old or Middle Chinese (especially Literary Chine ...
'' vocabulary is usually not affected by rendaku when it occurs as the second element of a compound, as illustrated by the contrast between 'glass shelf', from native Japanese 'shelf', and 'glass case', from foreign 'case': rendaku does not occur and is not expected to occur in the latter compound word, since its non-initial element belongings to foreign vocabulary. (In contrast, as shown by , the use of a ''gairaigo'' word as the first element of a compound does not prevent rendaku of a following native element.) Examples where rendaku affects a ''gairaigo'' element of a compound are highly exceptional.


Part of speech

Rendaku affects compounds involving various parts of speech, such as Noun + Verb, Adjective + Noun, Verb + Noun, Verb + Adjective; however, it is rare in compounds of the form Verb + Verb.


Reduplication

Rendaku is very frequent in words formed by
reduplication In linguistics, reduplication is a Morphology (linguistics), morphological process in which the Root (linguistics), root or Stem (linguistics), stem of a word, part of that, or the whole word is repeated exactly or with a slight change. The cla ...
, other than mimetic words, which are immune to rendaku. Examples: : (
iteration Iteration is the repetition of a process in order to generate a (possibly unbounded) sequence of outcomes. Each repetition of the process is a single iteration, and the outcome of each iteration is then the starting point of the next iteration. ...
) :: ''hito'' + ''hito'' → ''hitobito'' ("person" + "person" → "people") : (iteration) :: ''toki'' + ''toki'' → ''tokidoki'' ("time" + "time" → "sometimes")


Semantics

''Rendaku'' tends not to occur in non-reduplicative compounds which have the
semantic Semantics is the study of linguistic Meaning (philosophy), meaning. It examines what meaning is, how words get their meaning, and how the meaning of a complex expression depends on its parts. Part of this process involves the distinction betwee ...
value of "X and Y" (so-called
dvandva A dvandva ('pair' in Sanskrit) is a linguistic compound in which multiple individual nouns are concatenated to form an agglomerated compound word in which the conjunction has been elided to form a new word with a distinct semantic field. For ins ...
or copulative compounds), as exemplified by ''yama'' + ''kawa'' > ''yamakawa'' "mountains and rivers", as opposed to ''yama'' + ''kawa'' > ''yamagawa'' "mountain river".


Branching compounds

In compounds containing more than two elements (or compounds where one element is itself a compound), the branching structure of the compound may affect the application of rendaku. For example, , a compound of the morphemes "door" and "shelf", retains its initial voiceless when used as the second element of the compound "small cupboard". In examples like this, where the second element contains a voiced consonant as a result of rendaku, the lack of voicing at the start of the second element of the larger compound can potentially be explained as a consequence of Lyman's law. proposed that rendaku is blocked in general in the left-branching elements of a right-branching compound, even in cases where Lyman's law does not apply. However, other linguists have questioned the validity or necessity of formulating such a constraint. The branching constraint is intended to explain the contrasting behavior of examples such as the following: :''mon'' + 'shiro'' + ''chō''> ''monshirochō'', not *''monjirochō'' ("
family crest A crest is a component of a heraldry, heraldic display, consisting of the device borne on top of the Helmet (heraldry), helm. Originating in the decorative sculptures worn by knights in tournament (medieval), tournaments and, to a lesser exten ...
" + white" + "butterfly"> " cabbage butterfly") but : 'o'' + ''shiro''+ ''washi'' > ''ojirowashi'' ( tail" + "white"+ "eagle" > "
white-tailed eagle The white-tailed eagle (''Haliaeetus albicilla''), sometimes known as the 'sea eagle', is a large bird of prey, widely distributed across temperate Eurasia. Like all eagles, it is a member of the family Accipitridae (or accipitrids) which also ...
") This constraint does not apply to all words where the second element is composed of more than one morpheme. As discussed above, rendaku can affect Sino-Japanese "binoms" composed of two Sino-Japanese roots. Assuming the branching constraint is valid, it is possible it does not prevent rendaku in that context because Sino-Japanese binoms do not have the morphological status of compound words in the context of rendaku. In any case, there seem to be some counterexamples to the branching constraint, such as 'big talk', from 'big' + , from ' bath' + 'carpet', or 'fire brigade for common people', from 'town' + , from 'fire' + 'to extinguish'. Other examples where rendaku appears to affect "multi-root" elements that are themselves composed of smaller elements (at least in terms of etymology) include the following: * () 'tavern', from 'staying' + 'saké-shop', from 'saké-' + '-shop' * () 'socializing with people', from 'person' + ()'socializing', from 'attaching' + 'matching' * () 'skirmish', from 'small' + 'competition', from 'vying' + 'matching' * () 'sole (fish)', from 'tongue' + 'flounder', from 'flat' + 'eye' Otsu accounted for apparent counterexamples to the branching rule by postulating a distinction between "loose" and "strict" compounds. Per Otsu, loose compounds are formed in a productive manner, have a predictable meaning based on their components, and follow the general accentuation rules for compound words. Loose compounds are hypothesized to be immune to rendaku when used as the right-hand element of a larger compound, whereas "strict" compounds can undergo rendaku the same way as single morphemes. However, Vance 2022 argues it is not clear that these criteria can really be applied reliably to predict whether a compound is "loose" or "strict" for the purposes of rendaku. The branching constraint analysis could be considered a violation of the Atom Condition, which states that "in lexical derivations from X, only features realized on X are accessible." An alternative view proposes that the process applies cyclically. : 'nuri'' + ''hashi''+ ''ire'' > ''nuribashiire'' ( acquered chopstickcase, "case for lacquered chopsticks") :''nuri'' + 'hashi'' + ''ire''> ''nurihashiire'' (lacquered hopstick case "lacquered case for chopsticks") This could be seen as the voicing between hashi and ire staying unrealized but still activating Lyman's Law. Ito and Mester 2003 proposed a third account distinct from both Otsu 1980's branching-based constraint and Ito and Mester 1986's cyclical account. This hypothesis holds that the relationship between branching and rendaku is not direct, but is mediated by
prosodic In linguistics, prosody () is the study of elements of speech, including intonation (linguistics), intonation, stress (linguistics), stress, Rhythm (linguistics), rhythm and loudness, that occur simultaneously with individual phonetic segments: v ...
structure: the lack of rendaku in right-branching compounds such as is analyzed as a consequence of coming at the start of a prosodic word. Per Ito and Mester 2007, whether a compound is treated as one or as multiple prosodic words is affected by the length of the second element of the compound: if the second element is longer than four moras (or two bimoraic feet), then the compound is required to have the prosody of a phrase rather than a single word.


Further considerations

In some cases, ''rendaku'' varies depending on syntax. For instance, the suffix , from , is pronounced as following the perfective verb, as in , but is pronounced as when following a noun, as in or, semantically differently – more concretely – . Most Japanese family names are compounds, and rendaku may or may not affect the second element of a compound name. Some names are read in different ways for different people, and have both a reading with rendaku and one without, such as (which can be either or , among other readings).


Lexical propensity

It has been hypothesized that morphemes are inherently either more susceptible or more resistant to being voiced when used as the second element of a compound; in other words, that the propensity of a morpheme to undergo rendaku is lexically specified. Some morphemes show voicing in all compounds in which they occur. At the far end of the spectrum, a small percentage of morphemes appear to be immune to rendaku (for reasons not explained by the phonological, morphosyntactic, and lexical-stratum factors described above that regularly block rendaku). Other morphemes are intermediate and undergo voicing in some but not all compounds. found that out of elements that occurred at least 5 times in second position in a database of frequent compound words, the most common behavior (42%) was to undergo voicing in all compounds, and each range of greater resistance to rendaku contained successively fewer elements (35% had voicing rates from 0.667 to 0.999; 12% had voicing rates from 0.334 to 0.666; 7% had voicing rates from 0.001 to 0.333, and 5% were never voiced). suggests that the first element of compound words also has a lexically variable propensity to trigger rendaku, hypothesizing that rendaku occurs in words where the combination of these two voicing weights (the rendaku-triggering weight of the first element and the rendaku-undergoing weight of the second element) exceeds a threshold value, and fails to occur when this value falls below the threshold.


Word length

In some circumstances, rendaku appears to be affected by the length of a compound word, as measured in moras. Certain second elements that have a length of one or two moras tend to resist rendaku when combined with a first element that is one or two moras long. (A compound where both elements are short can be called a "short-short compound"). For example, occurs in a number of compounds without rendaku, such as and . However, these resistant elements undergo rendaku when combined with a first element that is more than two moras long, as in .


Avoidance of bVmV

Rendaku seems to be avoided in non-verbal elements that start with or + vowel + + vowel, such as , which has been explained as an effect of a preference against having
homorganic In phonetics, a homorganic consonant (from Latin and ) is a consonant sound that is articulated in the same place of articulation as another. For example, , and are homorganic consonants of one another since they share the bilabial place of ...
consonants at the start of adjacent syllables: rendaku would replace or with , which is bilabial like . Rendaku is seen before vowel + + vowel in 'love letter' from 'letter': this is consistent with the hypothesis, since contains , which is already homorganic with (and so avoiding rendaku would not make any difference). (An alternative explanation supposes that , as well as other words that undergo rendaku to , starts with an underlying labial consonant that is phonologically distinct from the found at the start of lexemes that do not undergo rendaku.) A study observed this tendency as an active factor affecting the frequency with which Japanese speakers preferred rendaku vs. non-rendaku variants of
nonce word In linguistics, a nonce word—also called an occasionalism—is any word (lexeme), or any sequence of sounds or letters, created for a single occasion or utterance but not otherwise understood or recognized as a word in a given languag ...
s. For whatever reason, this avoidance is not seen in verb roots (including deverbal nouns), which undergo rendaku even in cases such as 'to corrupt'. No comparable effect is seen with other consonant sounds that undergo rendaku, since they retain the same place of articulation after the change.


Productivity

Although rendaku does not occur in all compounds, it is common and studies of Japanese native speakers show that it occurs often in newly formed compounds. It has been suggested that its productivity in new compounds is caused by analogy with existing compound words.


Acquisition by children

Some studies have examined the
phonological development Phonological development refers to how children learn to organize sounds into meaning or language (phonology) during their stages of growth. Sound is at the beginning of language learning. Children have to learn to distinguish different sounds and ...
of rendaku. Studies from the late 1990s and early 2000s indicate that children under five and a half years old show limited use of rendaku in any context. The use of rendaku in established compounds becomes frequent by the age of six, but children below this age do not show reliable use of rendaku in unfamiliar compounds. Children over the age of six use rendaku frequently both in familiar and unfamiliar compounds.


Theoretical analysis

In linguistics, rendaku has been analyzed in terms of various
phonological Phonology (formerly also phonemics or phonematics: "phonemics ''n.'' 'obsolescent''1. Any procedure for identifying the phonemes of a language from a corpus of data. 2. (formerly also phonematics) A former synonym for phonology, often prefer ...
theories. It has been popular to identify Lyman's Law, the restriction against applying rendaku to a morpheme that already contains a voiced obstruent phoneme, as an example of a more general theoretical concept known as the obligatory contour principle (or "OCP" for short). This principle was originally formulated (by Leben 1973) to refer to the phonology of tone, and referred to a hypothesized constraint against having consecutive identical tones in the underlying representation of a morpheme. Later phonologists have interpreted the principle as a more general constraint, using it to refer also to bans on identical adjacent specifications of various non-tone features. Thus, Lyman's Law has been interpreted as an example where the obligatory contour principle (OCP) applies to voicing specifications and rules out multiple occurrences of voicing within a morpheme.


Rendaku in Tohoku dialects

In the Kahoku dialect of Tōhoku, the phonemes have voiced allophones when they come between voiced vowels, and the phonemes have prenasalized or nasal allophones when they come between vowels: for example, 'target' is pronounced (with a second consonant that is voiced but not prenasalized) whereas 'window' is pronounced (with both voicing and prenasalization of the second consonant). Thus, rendaku in this dialect, when it occurs, typically involves phonetic prenasalization of the second element of the compound, as is speculated to have been the case in prehistoric Japanese: e.g. 'beach" + 'chestnut' → 'clam'. However, the use of prenasalization at the start of the second element of a compound word is not uniform, and depending on the speakers and the words pronounced, significant variations were observed. There was a relationship between the rate of prenasalization and the speakers’ age: older individuals use prenasalized pronunciations at a higher rate than younger individuals. On the other hand, differences in the speakers’ gender and socioeconomic status did not affect the rate of prenasalization. It is speculated that dialect mixing with standard Japanese may have increased the use of pronunciations without prenasalization. In the case of , loss of prenasalization may also have additionally been facilitated by the fact that the resulting sound did not merge with any preexisting phoneme, since does not occur intervocalically in non-mimetic vocabulary (in contrast, in forms like e̝ɡɑmɨ the absence of prenasalization makes it possible to interpret the consonant as merely the voiced intervocalic allophone of , which would result in the phonemic transcription , with the same consonant phoneme as the independent word ). A Tōhoku dialect spoken in
Iwate Prefecture is a Prefectures of Japan, prefecture of Japan located in the Tōhoku region of Honshu. It is the second-largest Japanese prefecture (behind Hokkaido) at , with a population of 1,165,886 (as of July 1, 2023). Iwate Prefecture borders Aomori Pre ...
has been reported to have a phonotactic constraint prohibiting prenasalized obstruents from occurring in two successive syllables; this rule produces regular denasalization in 'ceremonial fire' as opposed to 'fireworks'., citing .


See also

*
Consonant mutation Consonant mutation is change in a consonant in a word according to its morphological or syntactic environment. Mutation occurs in languages around the world. A prototypical example of consonant mutation is the initial consonant mutation of al ...
*
Lenition In linguistics, lenition is a sound change that alters consonants, making them "weaker" in some way. The word ''lenition'' itself means "softening" or "weakening" (from Latin 'weak'). Lenition can happen both synchronically (within a language ...
*
Sandhi Sandhi ( ; , ) is any of a wide variety of sound changes that occur at morpheme or word boundaries. Examples include fusion of sounds across word boundaries and the alteration of one sound depending on nearby sounds or the grammatical function o ...


Notes


References


Bibliography

* * (Japanese citation: ) * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * Japanese citation: * * * * * * * * * (Japanese citation: )


Further reading

* *


External links


The Japanese Lexicon: A Rendaku Encyclopedia
National Institute for Japanese Language and Linguistics The (NINJAL) is an independent administrative institution in Japan, established for the purpose of studying, surveying, promoting, and making recommendations for the proper usage of the Japanese language.NINJALweb page (English)/ref> The insti ...
{{Japanese language Japanese phonology Japanese writing system terms