Ute
[Givón, T. ''Ute Reference Grammar''. John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2011] is a dialect of the
Colorado River Numic language, spoken by the
Ute people
Ute () are the Indigenous people of the Ute tribe and culture among the Indigenous peoples of the Great Basin. They had lived in sovereignty in the regions of present-day Utah and Colorado in the Southwestern United States for many centuries unt ...
. Speakers primarily live on three reservations:
Uintah-Ouray (or Northern Ute) in northeastern Utah,
Southern Ute in southwestern
Colorado
Colorado (, other variants) is a state in the Mountain states, Mountain West subregion of the Western United States. It encompasses most of the Southern Rocky Mountains, as well as the northeastern portion of the Colorado Plateau and the wes ...
, and
Ute Mountain
Ute Mountain, also known as Ute Peak or Sleeping Ute Mountain (; Ute: ''Wisuv Káruv'', Navajo: ''Dził Naajiní''), is a peak within the Ute Mountains, a small mountain range in the southwestern corner of Colorado. It is on the northern edge o ...
in southwestern Colorado and southeastern
Utah
Utah ( , ) is a state in the Mountain West subregion of the Western United States. Utah is a landlocked U.S. state bordered to its east by Colorado, to its northeast by Wyoming, to its north by Idaho, to its south by Arizona, and to its ...
.
Ute is part of the
Numic branch of the
Uto-Aztecan
Uto-Aztecan, Uto-Aztekan or (rarely in English) Uto-Nahuatl is a family of indigenous languages of the Americas, consisting of over thirty languages. Uto-Aztecan languages are found almost entirely in the Western United States and Mexico. The na ...
language family.
Other dialects in this
dialect chain are Chemehuevi and Southern Paiute. As of 2010, there were 1,640 speakers combined of all three dialects Colorado River Numic.
Ute's parent language, Colorado River Numic, is classified as a threatened language, although there are tribally-sponsored language revitalization programs for the dialect.
''Ute'' as a term was applied to the group by Spanish explorers, being derived from the term ''quasuatas'', used by the Spanish at the time to refer to all tribes north of the Pueblo peoples and up to the Shoshone peoples.
The Ute people refer to their own language as ''núu-'apaghap
i'' or ''núuchi'', meaning "the people's speech" and "of the people" respectively.
Phonology
Vowels
T.Givon (2011) gives the following orthography and phonetic information for Southern Ute. Northern Ute differs from Southern and Central in some lexical and phonological areas.
Southern Ute has five vowels, as well as several allophones, which are not shown in the orthography. Each vowel can be short or long, and vowel length is marked orthographically by doubling the vowel. In Ute, the length of a vowel is often phonemic, and relevant for determining meaning. For example, ''whca-y'', meaning 'wrapping,' versus ''whcáa-y'', meaning 'swirling'. In some cases, however, the difference between a long and a short vowel is purely phonetic, and does not change word meaning. Ute devoices vowels in certain phonological or grammatical environments, as described in later sections. Devoiced vowels are marked in the orthography by underlining them, or, when the identity of the underlying vowel has been lost, with the letter
Here bold text indicates a practical orthographic representation, while the
IPA
IPA commonly refers to:
* India pale ale, a style of beer
* International Phonetic Alphabet, a system of phonetic notation
* Isopropyl alcohol, a chemical compound
IPA may also refer to:
Organizations International
* Insolvency Practitioner ...
representation is included in brackets.
Allophones
* is an allophone of ʉ
* and are both allophones of a; the former is used more often by younger speakers, while older speakers use the latter
* is an allophone of ɵ.
Consonants
Southern Ute consonants are given in the table below. As above, orthographic representations are bold and the IPA representations are in brackets. All stops in Ute are voiceless. Thus, g here does not indicate a
voiced velar stop but rather a
voiced velar fricative
The voiced velar fricative is a type of consonantal sound that is used in various spoken languages. It is not found in Modern English but existed in Old English. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is ...
, similar to ''luego'' in Spanish. Also similar to Spanish is the
voiced bilabial fricative
The voiced bilabial fricative is a type of consonantal sound, used in some Speech communication, spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is , and the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is B. The offi ...
v, as in the Spanish phrase ''la verdad'', in contrast with the voiced labiodental fricative which does not appear in Ute. The velar sounds k and g have
uvular allophones
In phonology, an allophone (; from the Greek , , 'other' and , , 'voice, sound') is a set of multiple possible spoken soundsor ''phones''or signs used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language. For example, in English, (as in ''s ...
: k becomes either a
voiceless uvular stop
The voiceless uvular plosive or stop is a type of consonantal sound, used in some spoken languages. It is pronounced like a voiceless velar plosive , except that the tongue makes contact not on the soft palate but on the uvula. The symbol in t ...
or a
voiceless uvular fricative
The voiceless uvular fricative is a type of consonantal sound used in some spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is , the Greek chi. The sound is represented by (ex with underdot) in A ...
when either between two vowels or adjacent to the vowel ; likewise g becomes a voiced uvular fricative under the same conditions. Either k or g can become a
voiceless velar fricative
The voiceless velar fricative is a type of consonantal sound used in some spoken languages. It was part of the consonant inventory of Old English and can still be found in some dialects of English, most notably in Scottish English, e.g. in ''l ...
when before a de-voiced word ending.
Note here that
coronals are produced as dental sounds rather than the
alveolar sounds used in English.
Allophones
* , and qh (or ), are allophones of k
* kh (or ), is an allophone of k or g
* gh, or , is an allophone of g
Syllable Structure
Syllables usually follow the CVCV pattern. All words must begin with a consonant, but other syllables may or may not include an onset. When an onset is present, it is usually composed of only one consonant. Words with suffixes like ''-'ni, -'na,'' and ''
'wa'', can have a two-consonant onset, though they were historically -''ni-'i, -na-'a,'' and ''-wa-'a'' respectively. These earlier suffix forms did have single-consonant onsets. Most syllables do not have codas, but some codas do appear at word-end, such as in ''pʉi-n'', 'I'm sleeping'.
Stress
Each Southern Ute word must have one
stressed vowel. Either the first or second vowel of a word in Ute may be stressed, with the latter situation being the most common. Stress is orthographically marked when it occurs on the first vowel. In compound words, the primary stress is applied to the first stem, and a secondary stress may also occur on a later stem.
Vowel stress is contrastive in pairs such as, ''suwá'', meaning 'almost', and ''súwa'', meaning 'straight out'.
Note that the
high back unrounded vowel ʉ often is pronounced as a high central when unstressed. Though this change produces some
minimal pair
In phonology, minimal pairs are pairs of words or phrases in a particular language, spoken or signed, that differ in only one phonological element, such as a phoneme, toneme or chroneme, and have distinct meanings. They are used to demonstrate ...
s, it is the destressing, rather than the vowel change, that produces the change in meaning and thus is excluded from the orthography.
Phonological processes
Ute has several phonological processes that affect the realization of underlying phonemes. Below is a representative sample.
* a changes to
or
�(usually for younger and older speakers respectively) when near y, i, or ɵ, such as in ''ɵæ-qar
u,'' 'yellow', or ''
'ura-'æ''y'','' 'is'. Although a often makes the alternation when directly preceding or following y, i, or ɵ, it does not have to be directly next to one of those phonemes, such as in ''sinaæv
i'', 'wolf'
* ɵ becomes
when directly preceding or following
� or
���however, k becomes
and
hbetween two as or directly preceding or following
so the precise mechanism is unknown. ''qhoqh'', 'bull-snake', is one word where this process occurs
* g becomes
�when between two as or directly preceding or following
such as in ''pagha-'ni,'' 'walking about'
* w is inserted after g and k if the g or k directly follow u,
or ɵ, such as in ''tagu-kwa'', 's/he was thirsty'
* vowels are sometimes devoiced in unstressed word-initial or word-final syllables, or unstressed syllables that begin with a voiceless consonant, nasal consonant, or glide, such as in ''whcaay'', 'swirl'
Morphology
Ute is
polysynthetic. Affixes are mostly
suffixes, but there are three major types of
prefix
A prefix is an affix which is placed before the stem of a word. Adding it to the beginning of one word changes it into another word. For example, when the prefix ''un-'' is added to the word ''happy'', it creates the word ''unhappy''. Particu ...
es for verbs and one for nouns.
Nouns
Most nouns in Ute obligatorily have suffixes. Inanimate nouns usually take the suffix -''p
u''/''-v
u''. However, this suffix can also sometimes denote animate nouns or body parts. Animate nouns usually take the ''-chi'' suffix, but can also take ''-vi/-pi'' or ''-t
u/-r
u''. The consonant pairs p/v and t/r were once allophones, but are no longer predictable; this produces the suffixes separated by a slash. Some older animate nouns have a silent final vowel rather than an explicit suffix.
-''p
u'' is also used to derive inanimate nouns from verbs, such as ''piki''-''p
u'' "rotten thing" from ''piki-'' "be rotten". ''-t
u/-r
u'' are used for animate nouns that derive from verbs or possession: thus, ''kaá-mi-t
u'' "singer" derives from ''kaá-mi
ya'' "sings" and ''piwa-gha-t
u'' "married person, spouse" derives from ''piwa-n'' "my spouse".
There are three ways plurality can be marked, and only animate nouns are marked for plurality. ''-u'' is the most common plural suffix, and ''-mu'' is usually used for plural nouns that derive from verbs or possession. These suffixes are placed after the obligatory noun suffix. Finally, some nouns show plurality by
reduplication of the first syllable in combination with the ''-u'' suffix, such as in ''táa-ta'wa-chi-u'' "men" from ''ta'wa-ch
i.'' In this case, ''-u'' without reduplication would create the dual form: "two men".
Verbs
Ute verbs can take many suffixes and several prefixes. Negation is marked with both the suffix ''-wa'' and prefix ''ka-''. Alternatively, instead of the prefix, the full form ''kách-'' can appear as a separate word somewhere before the verb being negated.
First syllable reduplication in verbs denotes the
distributive case
The distributive case ( abbreviated ) is used on nouns for the meanings of ''per'' or ''each.''
In Hungarian it is ''-nként'' and expresses the manner when something happens to each member of a set one by one (e.g., ''fejenként'' "per head", '' ...
. Thus, ''táa-p
ugay-'u'' "
/hekicked him (once)" becomes ''t
a-táa-p
ugay-'u'' "
/hekicked him repeatedly".
Incorporation
Incorporation may refer to:
* Incorporation (business), the creation of a corporation
* Incorporation of a place, creation of municipal corporation such as a city or county
* Incorporation (academic), awarding a degree based on the student having ...
can take place at the leftmost prefix position to add the meanings of the incorporated word to the verb. For example, ''
'apagha-y'' "
/heis talking" and ''pia-'apagha-y'' "
/heis sweet-talking".
Verbs usually take the suffix''-ka'' after the stem when the subject is plural. ''-ka'' can also be realized as ''-qa, -kwa, -kya'', etc. according to the phonological processes above. Many suffixes are used to denote
tense, aspect, and modality. Some of the more common of these suffixes include ''-y'' for the present tense, ''-vaa-ni'' for the future, and ''-mi
ya''. for the habitual. Other suffixes include ''-ti, -k
u,'' and ''-ta'', which mark the causative,
benefactive
The benefactive case (abbreviated , or sometimes when it is a core argument) is a grammatical case used where English would use "for", "for the benefit of", or "intended for", e.g. "She opened the door ''for Tom''" or "This book is ''for Bob'' ...
, and
passive case respectively.
Syntax
Word order in Ute is flexible and determined primarily by discourse pragmatics, although speakers will mostly use SOV order when producing isolated clauses.
Case Marking
Ute marks nouns for nominative and oblique case. The former category contains subjects and predicates, and the latter contains objects and genitives. In most cases, the final vowel of the entire noun is devoiced in the nominative case and voiced in the oblique case. For example, "woman" in the nominative is ''mama-ch
i'' and in the oblique is ''mama-chi.'' In some pronouns, the (voiced) suffix ''-y'' is added to mark the oblique case, as in singular "you", which is ''
'
úm
u'' in the nominative and ''
'
úm
u-y'' in the oblique.
Noun Incorporation
As described above in morphology, nouns and other words can be incorporated as prefixes of verbs to specify the method of action: for example, ''wii-chi-m t
uka-y-aqh'', "s/he eats it with a knife" can incorporate ''wii-chi-m,'' "knife", into the verb ''t
uka-y-aqh'', "eats" to produce ''wii-t
uka-y-aqh'', "s/he is knife-eating it".
Switch Reference
Switch reference uses the independent pronoun ''
'uwas'', "s/he", or ''
'um
us'', "they", to refer to a previously-introduced subject when there are multiple previously-introduced parties, to indicate that the subject of the current clause is different from the previously-mentioned subject. For example, in ''
'áa-gha máy-kya-p
ugay-k
u, 'ú-vwaa pagha'ni-p
uga
'uwas'', "as they were whispering (amongst themselves), he paced around there", when the sentence begins, the subject is "they", and the independent pronoun is used when the subject changes to "he", a previously introduced character.
Notes
{{Ute people
Numic languages
Agglutinative languages
Indigenous languages of the United States
Paiute
Ute tribe