HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Ute ( )Givón, T. ''Ute Reference Grammar''. John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2011 is a dialect of the
Colorado River Numic language Colorado River Numic (also called Ute , Southern Paiute , Ute–Southern Paiute, or Ute-Chemehuevi ), of the Numic branch of the Uto-Aztecan language family, is a dialect chain that stretches from southeastern California to Colorado. Individ ...
, spoken by the
Ute people Ute () are an Indigenous peoples of the Great Basin, Indigenous people of the Great Basin and Colorado Plateau in present-day Utah, western Colorado, and northern New Mexico.Pritkzer''A Native American Encyclopedia'' p. 242 Historically, their t ...
. Speakers primarily live on three reservations: Uintah-Ouray (or Northern Ute) in northeastern Utah, Southern Ute in southwestern
Colorado Colorado is a U.S. state, state in the Western United States. It is one of the Mountain states, sharing the Four Corners region with Arizona, New Mexico, and Utah. It is also bordered by Wyoming to the north, Nebraska to the northeast, Kansas ...
, and
Ute Mountain Ute Mountain, also known as Ute Peak or Sleeping Ute Mountain (; Ute: ''Wisuv Káruv'', Navajo: ''Dził Naajiní''), is a peak within the Ute Mountains, a small mountain range in the southwestern corner of Colorado. It is on the northern edge o ...
in southwestern Colorado and southeastern
Utah Utah is a landlocked state in the Mountain states, Mountain West subregion of the Western United States. It is one of the Four Corners states, sharing a border with Arizona, Colorado, and New Mexico. It also borders Wyoming to the northea ...
. Ute is part of the Numic branch of the
Uto-Aztecan The Uto-Aztecan languages are a family of native American languages, consisting of over thirty languages. Uto-Aztecan languages are found almost entirely in the Western United States and Mexico. The name of the language family reflects the common ...
language family. Other dialects in this
dialect chain A dialect continuum or dialect chain is a series of language varieties spoken across some geographical area such that neighboring varieties are mutually intelligible, but the differences accumulate over distance so that widely separated variet ...
are Chemehuevi and Southern Paiute. As of 2010, there were 1,640 speakers combined of all three dialects Colorado River Numic. Ute's parent language, Colorado River Numic, is classified as a threatened language, although there are tribally-sponsored language revitalization programs for the dialect. ''Ute'' as a term was applied to the group by Spanish explorers, being derived from the term ''quasuatas'', used by the Spanish at the time to refer to all tribes north of the Pueblo peoples and up to the Shoshone peoples. The Ute people refer to their own language as or , meaning "the people's speech" and "of the people" respectively.


Phonology


Vowels

T.Givon (2011) gives the following orthography and phonetic information for Southern Ute. Northern Ute differs from Southern and Central in some lexical and phonological areas. Southern Ute has five vowels, as well as several allophones, which are not shown in the orthography. Each vowel can be short or long, and vowel length is marked orthographically by doubling the vowel. In Ute, the length of a vowel is often phonemic, and relevant for determining meaning. For example, ''whca-y'', meaning 'wrapping', versus ''whcáa-y'', meaning 'swirling'. In some cases, however, the difference between a long and a short vowel is purely phonetic, and does not change word meaning. Ute devoices vowels in certain phonological or grammatical environments, as described in later sections. Devoiced vowels are marked in the orthography by underlining them, or, when the identity of the underlying vowel has been lost, with the letter Here bold text indicates a practical orthographic representation, while the IPA representation is included in brackets.


Allophones

* is an allophone of ʉ * and are both allophones of a; the former is used more often by younger speakers, while older speakers use the latter * is an allophone of ɵ.


Consonants

Southern Ute consonants are given in the table below. As above, orthographic representations are bold and the IPA representations are in brackets. All stops in Ute are voiceless. Thus, g here does not indicate a
voiced velar stop The voiced velar plosive or stop is a type of consonantal sound used in many spoken languages. Some languages have the voiced pre-velar plosive, which is articulated slightly more front compared with the place of articulation of the prototypic ...
but rather a
voiced velar fricative The voiced velar fricative is a type of consonantal sound that is used in various spoken languages. It is not found in most varieties of Modern English but existed in Old English. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents ...
, similar to ''luego'' in Spanish. Also similar to Spanish is the
voiced bilabial fricative The voiced bilabial fricative is a type of consonantal sound, used in some spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is , and the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is B. The official symbol is the ...
v, as in the Spanish phrase ''la verdad'', in contrast with the voiced labiodental fricative which does not appear in Ute. The velar sounds k and g have
uvular Uvulars are consonants articulated with the back of the tongue against or near the uvula, that is, further back in the mouth than velar consonants. Uvulars may be stops, fricatives, nasals, trills, or approximants, though the IPA does not prov ...
allophones In phonology, an allophone (; from the Greek , , 'other' and , , 'voice, sound') is one of multiple possible spoken soundsor '' phones''used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language. For example, in English, the voiceless plosi ...
: k becomes either a
voiceless uvular stop The voiceless uvular plosive or stop is a type of consonantal sound, used in some spoken languages. It is pronounced like a voiceless velar plosive , except that the tongue makes contact not on the soft palate but on the uvula. The symbol in ...
or a
voiceless uvular fricative The voiceless uvular fricative is a type of consonantal sound that is used in some spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is , the Greek chi. The sound is represented by (ex with underdo ...
when either between two vowels or adjacent to the vowel ; likewise g becomes a voiced uvular fricative under the same conditions. Either k or g can become a
voiceless velar fricative The voiceless velar fricative is a type of consonantal sound used in some spoken languages. It was part of the consonant inventory of Old English and can still be found in some dialects of English, most notably in Scottish English, e.g. in ''lo ...
when before a de-voiced word ending. Note here that coronals are produced as dental sounds rather than the
alveolar Alveolus (; pl. alveoli, adj. alveolar) is a general anatomical term for a concave cavity or pit. Uses in anatomy and zoology * Pulmonary alveolus, an air sac in the lungs ** Alveolar cell or pneumocyte ** Alveolar duct ** Alveolar macrophage * M ...
sounds used in English.


Allophones

* , and qh (or ), are allophones of k * kh (or ), is an allophone of k or g * gh, or , is an allophone of g


Syllable structure

Syllable A syllable is a basic unit of organization within a sequence of speech sounds, such as within a word, typically defined by linguists as a ''nucleus'' (most often a vowel) with optional sounds before or after that nucleus (''margins'', which are ...
s usually follow the CVCV pattern. All words must begin with a consonant, but other syllables may or may not include an onset. When an onset is present, it is usually composed of only one consonant. Words with suffixes like ''-'ni, -'na,'' and '''wa'', can have a two-consonant onset, though they were historically -''ni-'i, -na-'a,'' and ''-wa-'a'' respectively. These earlier suffix forms did have single-consonant onsets. Most syllables do not have codas, but some codas do appear at word-end, such as in ''pʉi-n'', 'I'm sleeping'.


Stress

Each Southern Ute word must have one stressed vowel. Either the first or second vowel of a word in Ute may be stressed, with the latter situation being the most common. Stress is orthographically marked when it occurs on the first vowel. In compound words, the primary stress is applied to the first stem, and a secondary stress may also occur on a later stem. Vowel stress is contrastive in pairs such as, ''suwá'', meaning 'almost', and ''súwa'', meaning 'straight out'. Note that the
high back unrounded vowel The close back unrounded vowel, or high back unrounded vowel, is a type of vowel sound used in some spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is . Typographically, it is a turned letter . ...
ʉ often is pronounced as a high central when unstressed. Though this change produces some
minimal pair In phonology, minimal pairs are pairs of words or phrases in a particular language, spoken or signed, that differ in only one phonological element, such as a phoneme, toneme or chroneme, and have distinct meanings. They are used to demonstrate t ...
s, it is the destressing, rather than the vowel change, that produces the change in meaning and thus is excluded from the orthography.


Phonological processes

Ute has several phonological processes that affect the realization of underlying phonemes. Below is a representative sample. * a changes to or (usually for younger and older speakers respectively) when near y, i, or ɵ, such as in ''ɵæ-qaru,'' 'yellow', or '''ura-'æ''y'','' 'is'. Although a often makes the alternation when directly preceding or following y, i, or ɵ, it does not have to be directly next to one of those phonemes, such as in ''sinaævi'', 'wolf' * ɵ becomes when directly preceding or following or ��however, k becomes and hbetween two as or directly preceding or following so the precise mechanism is unknown. ''qhoqh'', 'bull-snake', is one word where this process occurs * g becomes when between two as or directly preceding or following such as in ''pagha-'ni,'' 'walking about' * w is inserted after g and k if the g or k directly follow u, or ɵ, such as in ''tagu-kwa'', 's/he was thirsty' ** y is also inserted if directly follow i, such as in ''ini-kya'' 's/he did' * vowels are sometimes devoiced in unstressed word-initial or word-final syllables, or unstressed syllables that begin with a voiceless consonant, nasal consonant, or glide, such as in ''whcaay'', 'swirl'. In marginal pairs, they may be considered distinct, such as in ''tʉkápi'' 'food (nominative)' and ''tʉkápi̱'' 'food (accusative)'.


Morphology

Ute is
polysynthetic In linguistic typology, polysynthetic languages, formerly holophrastic languages, are highly synthetic languages, i.e., languages in which words are composed of many morphemes (word parts that have independent meaning but may or may not be able t ...
. Affixes are mostly
suffix In linguistics, a suffix is an affix which is placed after the stem of a word. Common examples are case endings, which indicate the grammatical case of nouns and adjectives, and verb endings, which form the conjugation of verbs. Suffixes can ca ...
es, but there are three major types of
prefix A prefix is an affix which is placed before the stem of a word. Particularly in the study of languages, a prefix is also called a preformative, because it alters the form of the word to which it is affixed. Prefixes, like other affixes, can b ...
es for verbs and one for nouns.


Nouns

Most nouns in Ute obligatorily have suffixes. Inanimate nouns usually take the suffix -''pu''/''-vu''. However, this suffix can also sometimes denote animate nouns or body parts. Animate nouns usually take the ''-chi'' suffix, but can also take ''-vi/-pi'' or ''-tu/-ru''. The consonant pairs p/v and t/r were once allophones, but are no longer predictable; this produces the suffixes separated by a slash. Some older animate nouns have a silent final vowel rather than an explicit suffix. -''pu'' is also used to derive inanimate nouns from verbs, such as ''piki''-''pu'' "rotten thing" from ''piki-'' "be rotten". ''-tu/-ru'' are used for animate nouns that derive from verbs or possession: thus, ''kaá-mi-tu'' "singer" derives from ''kaá-miya'' "sings" and ''piwa-gha-tu'' "married person, spouse" derives from ''piwa-n'' "my spouse". There are three ways plurality can be marked, and only animate nouns are marked for plurality. ''-u'' is the most common plural suffix, and ''-mu'' is usually used for plural nouns that derive from verbs or possession. These suffixes are placed after the obligatory noun suffix. Finally, some nouns show plurality by
reduplication In linguistics, reduplication is a Morphology (linguistics), morphological process in which the Root (linguistics), root or Stem (linguistics), stem of a word, part of that, or the whole word is repeated exactly or with a slight change. The cla ...
of the first syllable in combination with the ''-u'' suffix, such as in ''táa-ta'wa-chi-u'' "men" from ''ta'wa-chi.'' In this case, ''-u'' without reduplication would create the dual form: "two men".


Verbs

Ute verbs can take many suffixes and several prefixes. Negation is marked with both the suffix ''-wa'' and prefix ''ka-''. Alternatively, instead of the prefix, the full form ''kách-'' can appear as a separate word somewhere before the verb being negated. First syllable reduplication in verbs denotes the
distributive case The distributive case (abbreviated ) is used on nouns for the meanings of 'per' or 'each.' In Hungarian it is and expresses the manner when something happens to each member of a set one by one (e.g., "per head", "in some case"), or the freque ...
. Thus, ''táa-pugay-'u'' " /hekicked him (once)" becomes ''ta-táa-pugay-'u'' " /hekicked him repeatedly". Incorporation can take place at the leftmost prefix position to add the meanings of the incorporated word to the verb. For example, '''apagha-y'' " /heis talking" and ''pia-'apagha-y'' " /heis sweet-talking". Verbs usually take the suffix''-ka'' after the stem when the subject is plural. ''-ka'' can also be realized as ''-qa, -kwa, -kya'', etc. according to the phonological processes above. Many suffixes are used to denote tense, aspect, and modality. Some of the more common of these suffixes include ''-y'' for the present tense, ''-vaa-ni'' for the future, and ''-miya''. for the habitual. Other suffixes include ''-ti, -ku,'' and ''-ta'', which mark the causative,
benefactive The benefactive case (abbreviated , or sometimes when it is a core argument) is a grammatical case used where English would use "for", "for the benefit of", or "intended for", e.g. "She opened the door ''for Tom''" or "This book is ''for Bob' ...
, and
passive Passive may refer to: * Passive voice, a grammatical voice common in many languages, see also Pseudopassive * Passive language, a language from which an interpreter works * Passivity (behavior), the condition of submitting to the influence of ...
case respectively.


Syntax

Word order in Ute is flexible and determined primarily by discourse pragmatics, although speakers will mostly use SOV order when producing isolated clauses.


Case marking

Ute marks nouns for nominative and oblique case. The former category contains subjects and predicates, and the latter contains objects and genitives. In most cases, the final vowel of the entire noun is devoiced in the nominative case and voiced in the oblique case. For example, "woman" in the nominative is ''mama-chi'' and in the oblique is ''mama-chi.'' In some pronouns, the (voiced) suffix ''-y'' is added to mark the oblique case, as in singular "you", which is '''úmu'' in the nominative and '''úmu-y'' in the oblique.


Noun incorporation

As described above in morphology, nouns and other words can be incorporated as prefixes of verbs to specify the method of action: for example, ''wii-chi-m tuka-y-aqh'', "s/he eats it with a knife" can incorporate ''wii-chi-m,'' "knife", into the verb ''tuka-y-aqh'', "eats" to produce ''wii-tuka-y-aqh'', "s/he is knife-eating it".


Switch reference

Switch reference In linguistics, switch-reference (SR) describes any clause-level morpheme that signals whether certain prominent arguments in 'adjacent' clauses are coreferential. In most cases, it marks whether the subject of the verb in one clause is corefer ...
uses the independent pronoun '''uwas'', "s/he", or '''umus'', "they", to refer to a previously introduced subject when there are multiple previously introduced parties, to indicate that the subject of the current clause is different from the previously mentioned subject. For example, in '''áa-gha máy-kya-pugay-ku, 'ú-vwaa pagha'ni-puga 'uwas'', "as they were whispering (amongst themselves), he paced around there", when the sentence begins, the subject is "they", and the independent pronoun is used when the subject changes to "he", a previously introduced character.


Notes

{{Ute people Numic languages Agglutinative languages Indigenous languages of the United States Paiute Ute (ethnic group)