Completion criteria
A general rule to identify a TCU would be to look for a possible completion in an ongoing utterance. There are three criteria to determine what constitutes a TCU: * Intonationally complete: This means the utterance is at a possible point of completion with the hint of a falling tone, signalling the possible ending of the utterance. * Grammatically complete: This means the utterance is at a possible point of completion with a hint of its syntactic completeness, i.e. it signals the end of a sentence in terms of its grammatical structure. * Pragmatically complete: This means the utterance is seen to have possibly accomplished its purpose in response to the situation during a conversation. For example, the utterance "She is really" can be seen as pragmatically complete in response to the exclamation, "That girl is really smart!", in the sense that it functions as an agreement to the previous comment. Scholars in the field of conversation analysis have debated over the relative significance of the above three criteria as signifiers of the end of an utterance. There are, however, no conclusive answer to the discussion, and one can classify a TCU as such when observing either one (or more) of the above features.Transition relevance place
A transition relevance place (TRP) is a point of possible completion (or potential end) of an utterance (hence a TCU) where speaker change is a possible next action.Turn allocation
Each time a turn is over, speakers also have to decide who can talk next, and this is called turn allocation. The rules for turn allocation is commonly formulated in the same way as in the original Simplest Systematics paper, with 2 parts where the first consists of 3 elements: #* a. If the current speaker selects a next one to speak at the end of current TCU (by name, gaze or contextual aspects of what is said), the selected speaker has the right and obligation to speak next. #* b. If the current speaker does not select a next speaker, other potential speakers have the right to self-select (the first starter gets the turn) #* c. If options 1a and 1b have not been implemented, current speaker may continue with another TCU. # At the end of that TCU, the option system applies again. Some types of turns may require extra work before they can successfully take place. Speakers wanting a long turn, for example to tell a story or describe important news, must first establish that others will not intervene during the course of the telling through some form of preface and approval by the listener (a so-called ''go-ahead''). The preface and its associated go-ahead comprise a ''pre-sequence''. Conversations cannot be appropriately ended by 'just stopping', but require a special closing sequence.Classifications
Role types
There are four types of TCU categorized by the roles they play in the utterance: * Lexical TCU: e.g. "Yes", "There" * Phrasal TCU: e.g. "In the basket", "out of here" * Clausal TCU: e.g. "When I am free", "If I got the job" * Sentential TCU: e.g. "I am working on my thesis", "He has got my car"Unit design types
TCUs can be created or recognized via four methods, i.e. types of unit design: *Types of silence
Based on the turn-taking system, three types of silence may be distinguished: * Pause: A period of silence within a speaker's TCU, i.e. during a speaker's turn when a sentence is not finished. * Gap: A period of silence between turns, for example after a question has been asked and not yet answered * Lapse: A period of silence when no sequence or other structured activity is in progress: the current speaker stops talking, does not select a next speaker, and no one self selects. Lapses are commonly associated with visual or other forms of disengagement between speakers, even if these periods are brief.References
Applied linguistics Linguistics {{Comm-stub