In
linguistics
Linguistics is the scientific study of language. The areas of linguistic analysis are syntax (rules governing the structure of sentences), semantics (meaning), Morphology (linguistics), morphology (structure of words), phonetics (speech sounds ...
, switch-reference (SR) describes any clause-level
morpheme
A morpheme is any of the smallest meaningful constituents within a linguistic expression and particularly within a word. Many words are themselves standalone morphemes, while other words contain multiple morphemes; in linguistic terminology, this ...
that signals whether certain prominent arguments in 'adjacent'
clauses
In language, a clause is a Constituent (linguistics), constituent or Phrase (grammar), phrase that comprises a semantic predicand (expressed or not) and a semantic Predicate (grammar), predicate. A typical clause consists of a subject (grammar), ...
are
coreferential. In most cases, it marks whether the
subject of the verb in one clause is coreferent with that of the previous clause, or of a subordinate clause to the matrix (main) clause that is dominating it.
Meanings of switch-reference
The basic distinction made by a switch-reference system is whether the following clause has the same subject (SS) or a different subject (DS). That is known as canonical switch-reference. For purposes of switch-reference, subject is defined as it is for languages with a nominative–accusative alignment: a subject is the sole argument of an intransitive clause or the agent of a transitive one. It holds even in languages with a high degree of
ergativity.
The
Washo language
Washo (or Washoe; endonym ) is an endangered Native American language isolate spoken by the Washo on the California–Nevada border in the drainages of the Truckee and Carson Rivers, especially around Lake Tahoe. While there were only 20 el ...
of California and Nevada exhibits a switch-reference system. When the subject of one verb is the same as the subject of the following verb, the verb takes no switch-reference marker. However, if the subject of one verb differs from the subject of the following verb, the verb takes the "different subject" marker, -š: as displayed below
The
Seri language
Seri () is an indigenous language spoken by between 716La situación sociolingüística de la lengua seri en 2006. and 900 Seri people in Punta Chueca and El Desemboque, two villages on the coast of Sonora, Mexico. The language is generally con ...
of northwestern Mexico also has a switch-reference system which is similar in most ways to those of other languages except for one very salient fact: the relevant argument in a passive clause is not the superficial subject of the passive verb but rather the always unexpressed underlying subject. In clauses with
subject raising, it is the raised subject that is relevant.
Principles of switch-reference systems
There are four fundamental properties that any switch reference system, canonical and non-canonical, should satisfy.
[van Gijn 2016, p. 35.] Any system that does not have all these properties are categorically not switch reference:
* Switch reference systems have switch reference markers that track a specific, generalized role, are morphologically marked, and apply at the clause level.
* Switch reference systems have at least one marker indicating coreference and at least one marker indicating disjoint-reference.
* Switch reference markers encode mutually exclusive values whose semantics include identity versus non-identity, and prototypically indicate coreference or disjoint reference across clauses.
* The semantics of the predicate in the controlling clause do not constrain the use of switch reference.
Canonical switch-reference
A commonly used definition of canonical switch reference is that "switch-reference is an inflectional category of the verb, which indicates whether or not its subject is identical with the subject of some other verb." There are several formal properties that apply specifically to canonical switch reference systems. They include:
*Canonical switch-reference markers are mostly
inflectional affixes
In linguistics, an affix is a morpheme that is attached to a word stem to form a new word or word form. The main two categories are derivational and inflectional affixes. Derivational affixes, such as ''un-'', ''-ation'', ''anti-'', ''pre-'' et ...
on verbs. They can sometimes be separate from the verb, typically at clause boundaries, and for coordinations.
*Canonical switch-reference markers are in paradigmatic opposition. Same subject and different subject clauses are thought of as counterparts in discourse, so both of their verbs can have different morphology and can be found in different slots. Some languages allow both same subject and different subject marking on a single verb, and the clause structure could also differ between the two.
*Switch-reference markers are monomorphemic, but can also be expressed as a combination of morphemes rather than a single affix. For instance, same subject and different subject markers are often portmanteau with other markers. In
Kiowa
Kiowa ( ) or Cáuigú () people are a Native Americans in the United States, Native American tribe and an Indigenous people of the Great Plains of the United States. They migrated southward from western Montana into the Rocky Mountains in Colora ...
, the same subject marker ''gɔ̀'' is also used as a nominal coordinator.
*Switch-reference
pivots are generally subjects, but can also be non-subjects as determined by syntactic (e.g. subject/object), semantic (e.g. agent/patient) or discourse-based (e.g. topic) salience. Different markers can use different criteria in selection
*Switch-reference is obligatorily marked regardless of the person, number, and gender features of the switch reference pivots. Switch reference is used even in cases where the interpretation of two subjects is evident. This rule is a differentiating factor between switch reference and other similar systems such as
logophoricity
Logophoricity is a phenomenon of Binding (linguistics), binding relation that may employ a morphologically different set of anaphoric forms, in the context where the referent is an entity whose speech, thoughts, or feelings are being reported. T ...
.
*Syntactic dependency exists between the clause that hosts the switch-reference marker, and the clauses containing the switch reference pivots. If the marked clause is a chain clause, the control clause is the immediately adjacent clause. If the switch-reference marker is used in a subordinate clause, then the control clause is the matrix clause. There are sometimes intervening clauses (clause-skipping), and occasionally the control clause is not overt. As well, sometimes switch reference markers can be suffixed to coordinators of independent clauses.
Non-canonical switch-reference
Many languages exhibit non-canonical switch-reference, the co-referents of arguments other than the subject being marked by switch-reference. Here is an example from Kiowa:
In this case, the use of the same-subject marker ''gɔ'' rather than the switch-reference marker ''nɔ'' indicates that the two subjects wrote letters at the same time, to the same person, and with the same subject.
In addition, the nominative subject is not always marked by switch-reference. For instance, many clauses, including those with
impersonal or weather verbs, have no subject at all but can both bear and trigger switch-reference.
[Keine 2013, p. 771.]
Form of switch-reference markers
Switch-reference markers often carry additional meanings or are at least fused with connectives that carry them. For instance, a switch-reference marker might mark a different subject and sequential events.
Switch-reference markers often appear attached to verbs, but they are not a verbal category. They often appear attached to sentence-initial particles, sentence-initial recapitulative verbs, adverbial conjunctions ('when', 'because', etc.), or coordinators ('and' or 'but' though it seems never 'or'), relativizers ('which,'that'), or sentence
complementizer
In linguistics (especially generative grammar), a complementizer or complementiser (list of glossing abbreviations, glossing abbreviation: ) is a functional category (part of speech) that includes those words that can be used to turn a clause in ...
s ('that'). They can also appear as free morphemes or as differing agreement paradigms. However, most switch-reference languages are
subject–object–verb languages, with verbs as well as complementizers and conjunctions coming at the end of clauses. Therefore, switch-reference often appears attached to verbs, a fact that has led to the common but erroneous claim that switch-reference is a verbal category.
One certain typological fact about switch-reference is that switch-reference markers appear at the 'edges' of clauses. It is found at the edge of either a subordinate clause (referring to the matrix clause) or at the edge of a coordinate clause (referring to the previous clause). It is also very common in clause-chaining languages of
New Guinea
New Guinea (; Hiri Motu: ''Niu Gini''; , fossilized , also known as Papua or historically ) is the List of islands by area, world's second-largest island, with an area of . Located in Melanesia in the southwestern Pacific Ocean, the island is ...
, where it is found at the edge of medial clauses.
Switch-reference is also sensitive to syntactic structure. It can skip a clause that is string-adjacent (spoken one right after another) and refer to a matrix clause. For instance, in the configuration
[BC">[B.html" ;"title="[B">[BC, for which B and C are subordinate clauses to A, any switch-reference-marking on C refers to A, not B.
Different perspectives
Switch-reference is accounted for by many different explanations. These are some of the current theories:
Switch-reference as binding
Finer’s account of switch-reference is connected to a generalized version of Chomsky’s binding theory that also accounts for Ā-positions (non-argument positions). Switch-reference markers occupy the head of the complementizer phrase (CP), which is an Ā-position. Same subject markers are Ā-anaphors (reflexives and reciprocals) and different subject markers are Ā-pronominals (pronouns that are not reflexives or reciprocals). That is, same subject marking is used when the indices are identical, and different subject marking is used otherwise. Since the switch-reference markers are complementizer heads, their domain (smallest XP with a subject) necessarily includes the subject of the higher clause, which can then be (non-)coreferent with the switch-reference marker.
Déchaine & Wiltshko (2002) propose an explanation of switch-reference based on the DP/ΦP distinction (ΦP is their proposed intermediate projection between NP and DP that should be able to act like either of their distributions). Déchaine & Wiltshko note that the different subject markers are very similar to their corresponding same subject markers with some added morphology such as SS ''-ig'' vs. DS ''-ig
in'' in Amele language">Amele.
This suggests that same subject markers are bare ΦPs and different subject markers are full DPs containing a ΦP. Since different subject markers are essentially DPs, they are subject to Principle C and so cannot be coreferent with any antecedent. This forces a different-subject reading. Additionally, switch-reference is dependent on tense. Same subject marking occurs, and only subjects act as pivots for switch-reference, because switch-reference is mediated by tense.
Switch-reference as event (dis)continuity
The distribution of same subject and different subject markers do not always align with the coreference of the two subjects. Van Gijn (2016) provides a sentence in Central Pomo where the same subject marker ''-hi'' is used despite the subjects being distinct (see thematic coherence):
Stirling (1993) proposed that switch-reference is about the congruence of "eventualities". Referential continuity is just one aspect of this. She notes six pivots for SR systems:
* The classic referential (dis)continuity: This includes canonical switch-reference as defined by Haiman & Munro (1983) which tracks whether subjects are same or different.
*Agentivity of protagonist: When the syntactic subjects are co-referent, unexpected different subject marking may occur, but the agentivity value of that participant changes. When the syntactic subjects are not co-referent, unexpected same subject marking may occur, but the subject of the reference clause does not introduce a new agentive participant.
*Time of event
*Location of event
*Mood of the clause: Switch-reference marks the basic opposition in agreement between realized or non-realized events in some languages.
*Transition out of a cohesive sequence of events: different subject markers may be used to indicate an unexpected change in the course of events in languages such as Amele and Yankunytjatjara.
Same subject markers indicate ''identity'' while different subject markers do ''non-identity'', where identity is about agreement between “aspects of eventualities” and non-identity is disagreement in at least one of those parameters.
Switch-reference as coordination height
Keine (2013) also notes the inconsistency in the alignment of same subject and different subject markers with their subjects that may not actually be same or different. For example, in these two Zuni sentences, different subject marking is used despite the subjects being co-referent:
Different subject marking is used in Mesa Grande Diegueño (Yuman family) as well. This is unexpected because weather verbs do not project their own subjects, so there are no actual subjects that could be co-referent.
If subject reference completely explained the distribution of switch reference markers, these sentences should not occur. What Keine proposes instead is that the switch-reference markers ''are'' the different modes of spelling out the coordination. As well, switch-reference may exist clause-internally due to the coordination of low verbal projections. To note, switch reference existing clause-internally would have no issues with locality since indices and references are not being tracked across whole clauses.
Under Keine’s proposal, if two VPs are conjoined, then there is only one ''v''P and one external argument (i.e. one subject). This subject is then semantically interpreted as the subject of both VPs. The coordination marker used in this context ''is'' the same subject marker. Two ''v''Ps, yielding two external arguments, may also be conjoined. Each one is interpreted as the subject of its respective VP. Morphological differences and semantic properties are just consequences of the tree geometry of the coordination structure.
The Amele sentences below illustrate Keine's coordination height proposal:
::Same-subject marking:
::Different-subject marking:

Switch-reference as index agreement
Arregi & Hanink (2021) propose that the embedded C head agrees with the subject of the embedded clause, as well as the subject of the higher clause in referential index. The same subject and different subject markers are the morphological realization of the embedded C head. If the index values of both subjects differ, or if there is feature conflict, then C is morphologically realized as ''-š'', the different subject marker in Washo. If there is no feature conflict, then C is realized as ∅, the same subject marker in Washo. By extension, for any switch reference system, if the embedded and superordinate subjects have the same reference index, then embedded C is realized as the same subject marker. Likewise, if there is feature conflict instead, C is realized as the different subject marker.
Distribution of switch-reference
Switch reference is found in hundreds of languages in
North America
North America is a continent in the Northern Hemisphere, Northern and Western Hemisphere, Western hemispheres. North America is bordered to the north by the Arctic Ocean, to the east by the Atlantic Ocean, to the southeast by South Ameri ...
,
South America
South America is a continent entirely in the Western Hemisphere and mostly in the Southern Hemisphere, with a considerably smaller portion in the Northern Hemisphere. It can also be described as the southern Subregion#Americas, subregion o ...
,
Australia
Australia, officially the Commonwealth of Australia, is a country comprising mainland Australia, the mainland of the Australia (continent), Australian continent, the island of Tasmania and list of islands of Australia, numerous smaller isl ...
, New Guinea (particularly in the
Trans-New Guinea phylum, but not in many Papuan language families of northern New Guinea ), and the South Pacific. Typologies exist for North America, Australia, and New Guinea. The distribution of these systems has been determined via surveys and
typological studies.
Switch-reference tends to occur in geographical clusters spread over distinct language families. This system is suspected to spread through language contact, or
areal diffusion, which accounts for the fact that the morphological marking varies from one language to the next. For example,
Kiowa
Kiowa ( ) or Cáuigú () people are a Native Americans in the United States, Native American tribe and an Indigenous people of the Great Plains of the United States. They migrated southward from western Montana into the Rocky Mountains in Colora ...
is the only language in the
Kiowa-Tanoan
Tanoan ( ), also Kiowa–Tanoan or Tanoan–Kiowa, is a family of languages spoken by indigenous peoples in present-day New Mexico, Kansas, Oklahoma, and Texas.
Most of the languages – Tiwa languages, Tiwa (Taos, Picuris, Southern Tiwa), Te ...
family that uses switch reference, which can be explained by the migration history of the
Kiowa tribe and their close contact with the
Crow
A crow is a bird of the genus ''Corvus'', or more broadly, a synonym for all of ''Corvus''. The word "crow" is used as part of the common name of many species. The related term "raven" is not linked scientifically to any certain trait but is rathe ...
and
Comanche
The Comanche (), or Nʉmʉnʉʉ (, 'the people'), are a Tribe (Native American), Native American tribe from the Great Plains, Southern Plains of the present-day United States. Comanche people today belong to the List of federally recognized tri ...
tribes, both of which use switch-reference in their language. Particularly in North America, the
Uto-Aztecan
The Uto-Aztecan languages are a family of native American languages, consisting of over thirty languages. Uto-Aztecan languages are found almost entirely in the Western United States and Mexico. The name of the language family reflects the common ...
language family is thought to have been a source of major influence.
[van Gijn 2016, p. 43.]
Americas
Many indigenous languages in Western South America use switch-reference systems such as
Quechuan,
Uru, and
Chipaya in the Andes, and
Tacanan,
Panoan,
Barbacoan,
Tucanoan, and
Jivarona in the Amazon area.
Panoan languages are unique in the way they allow different coreference pivots such as transitive and intransitive subjects, as well as objects.
[van Gijn 2016, p. 44.]
In North America, there are 11 language families and 4 isolate languages that use this system. These native languages that feature switch-reference can be found in regions stretching from the south and south-west of the U.S. to the north-west of Mexico. These include the
Yuman–Cochimí,
Muskogean
Muskogean ( ; also Muskhogean) is a language family spoken in the Southeastern United States. Members of the family are Indigenous Languages of the Americas. Typologically, Muskogean languages are highly synthetic and agglutinative. One docume ...
,
Maiduan,
Pomoan,
Yokutsan,
Plateau Penutian,
Yukian, Kiowa-Tanoan,
Siouan, and the
Numic and
Takic
The Takic languages are a putative group of Uto-Aztecan languages historically spoken by a number of Indigenous peoples of California, Indigenous peoples of Southern California. Takic is grouped with the Tübatulabal language, Tubatulabal, Hopi la ...
(subgroups of Uto-Aztecan) language families, and the
Seri,
Tonkawa
The Tonkawa are a Native American tribe from Oklahoma and Texas. Their Tonkawa language, now extinct language, extinct, is a linguistic isolate. Today, Tonkawa people are enrolled in the Federally recognized tribes, federally recognized Tonkawa ...
,
Washo, and
Zuni isolates. These North American languages are unique in their productive use of this system, using switch-reference in coordinate, relative, and complement clauses, as well as semantically
underspecified clause chains.
Australia and New Guinea
Australian languages that use switch-reference include that aboriginal language families
Pama-Nyungan,
Arabana
The Arabana, also known as the Ngarabana, are an Aboriginal Australian people of South Australia.
Name
The older tribal autonym was Ngarabana, which may have been misheard by white settlers as Arabana, the term now is generally accepted by ne ...
-
Wangganguru,
Arandic,
Wagaya,
Garawa-
Waanyi, and
Djingili.
Further, 70% of
Papuan languages, referring to languages native to the island of
New Guinea
New Guinea (; Hiri Motu: ''Niu Gini''; , fossilized , also known as Papua or historically ) is the List of islands by area, world's second-largest island, with an area of . Located in Melanesia in the southwestern Pacific Ocean, the island is ...
, make use of switch reference systems.
[van Gijn 2016, p. 45.] While languages in Papua New Guinea are rich with personal pronouns, verbs still require switch-reference and agreement markers for participant tracking.
Other regions
Switch-reference systems are also present in languages of
Vanuatu
Vanuatu ( or ; ), officially the Republic of Vanuatu (; ), is an island country in Melanesia located in the South Pacific Ocean. The archipelago, which is of volcanic origin, is east of northern Australia, northeast of New Caledonia, east o ...
, parts of Africa, and potentially eastern Siberia.
Vanuatu languages are distinctive in that they mark the
anticipatory subject. Although Africa is not typically known to be a region with switch-reference, it is quite prevalent in
Omotic
The Omotic languages are a group of languages spoken in southwestern Ethiopia, in the Omo River region and southeastern Sudan in Blue Nile State. The Geʽez script is used to write some of the Omotic languages, the Latin script for some others. T ...
languages, particularly within the
North Omotic subgroup.
This influence may have also contributed to the development of switch-reference systems in
East Cushitic languages. Finally, the eastern Siberian
Yukaghir language family and
Even, a
Tungusic language, may be considered switch-reference languages but there is currently inconclusive evidence.
[van Gijn 2016, p. 46.]
Notes
{{reflist
References
*Arregi, K. & Hanink, E. A. (2021). Switch reference as index agreement. ''Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 40'', 651-702.
*Austin, P. (1981). Switch-Reference in Australia. ''Language, 57''(2), 309-334.
*Déchaine, R.-M. & Wiltschko, M. (2002). Decomposing pronouns. ''Linguistic Inquiry, 33''(3), 409-442.
*de Sousa, H. (2016). Some non-canonical switch reference systems and the fundamental functions of switch reference. In R. van Gijn & J. Hammond (Eds.), ''Switch Reference 2.0'' (pp. 55-92). John Benjamins.
*de Vries, L. (2017). Greater Awyu Languages of West Papua in Typological Perspective. In A. Y. Aikhenvald & R. M. W. Dixon (Eds.), ''The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Typology'' (pp. 942-996). Cambridge University Press.
*Farrell, P., Stephen, A. M., & Perlmutter, D. M. (1991). Notions of subjecthood and switch-reference: Evidence from Seri. ''Linguistic Inquiry, 22''(3), 431-456.
*Finer, D. (1985). The syntax of switch-reference. ''Linguistic Inquiry, 16''(1), 35-55.
*Foley, W. A. (2018). The morphosyntactic typology of Papuan languages. In B. Palmer (Ed.), ''The Languages and Linguistics of the New Guinea Area: A Comprehensive Guide'' (pp. 895-938). De Gruyter Mouton.
*Haiman, J. & Munro, P. (1983). Introduction. In J. Haiman & P. Munro (Eds.), ''Switch Reference and Universal Grammar'' (pp. ix-xv). John Benjamins.
*Jacobsen, W. H. (1983). Typological and Genetic Notes on Switch-Reference in North American Indian Languages. In J. Haiman & P. Munro (Eds.), ''Switch Reference and Universal Grammar'' (pp. 151-183). John Benjamins.
*Keine, S. (2013). Deconstructing switch-reference. ''Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 31'', 767-826.
*Mithun, M. (1999). ''The Languages of Native North America''. Cambridge University Press.
*McKenzie, A. (2015). A Survey of Switch-Reference in North America. ''International Journal of American Linguistics, 81''(3), 409-448.
*Marlett, S. A. (2020). Switch-reference and subject raising in Seri. In E.-D. Cook & D. Gerdts (Eds.), ''Syntax and semantics 16: the syntax of Native American Languages'' (pp. 247-268). Brill. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004373129
*Roberts, John (1997). ''Switch-Reference in Papua New Guinea'', 101–241. Number 3 in Papers in Papuan Linguistics. Canberra, ACT, Australia: Australian National University.
*Stirling, L. (1985). ''Switch-Reference and Logophoricity in Discourse Representation Theory''. Edinburgh University Press.
*van Gijn, R. (2016). Switch reference: an overview. In R. van Gijn & J. Hammond (Eds.), ''Switch Reference 2.0'' (pp. 1-53). John Benjamins.
*Watkins, L. J. (1993). The Discourse Functions of Kiowa Switch-Reference. ''International Journal of American Linguistics, 59''(2), 137-164.
Grammar
Ambiguity