HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Southeast Babar is an
Austronesian Austronesian may refer to: *The Austronesian languages *The historical Austronesian peoples The Austronesian people, sometimes referred to as Austronesian-speaking peoples, are a large group of peoples who have settled in Taiwan, maritime Sout ...
language spoken on Babar Island in South Maluku,
Indonesia Indonesia, officially the Republic of Indonesia, is a country in Southeast Asia and Oceania, between the Indian Ocean, Indian and Pacific Ocean, Pacific oceans. Comprising over List of islands of Indonesia, 17,000 islands, including Sumatra, ...
.


Phonology


Consonants

The following consonant inventory is provided by Steinhauer (2009). * Sounds only occur in loanwords. * is only attested in loanwords and also infrequently in roots. * /b/ and /d/ are marginal and only occur in a few words.


Other consonants in Taber's wordlist

A wordlist collected in a 1993 article by Mark Taber records words with extra distinct sounds that are not recorded nor accorded phonemic status in Steinhauer's study.


Vowels

* are heard as more closed when occurring before glides .


Phonotactics

Due to extensive syncope of vowels in both prefixes and original final syllables, Southeast Babar admits a wide variety of
consonant cluster In linguistics, a consonant cluster, consonant sequence or consonant compound is a group of consonants which have no intervening vowel. In English, for example, the groups and are consonant clusters in the word ''splits''. In the education fie ...
s both in the onsets and codas of syllables. One stark example of permitted consonant clusters is ''xweapk'' "we (inclusive) speak". Word-final clusters of a consonant followed by /j/ may be optionally subject to
epenthesis In phonology, epenthesis (; Greek ) means the addition of one or more sounds to a word, especially in the first syllable ('' prothesis''), the last syllable ('' paragoge''), or between two syllabic sounds in a word. The opposite process in whi ...
, with a non-phonemic being inserted either between the two consonants or after them. Both schwa positions can be used by the same speaker in
free variation In linguistics, free variation is the phenomenon of two (or more) sounds or forms appearing in the same environment without a change in meaning and without being considered incorrect by native speakers. Sociolinguists argue that describing such ...
.


Phonological history

Southeast Babar is notable for its drastic phonetic reshapings of inherited Austronesian vocabulary, with extensive consonant loss, unusual reflexes of surviving consonants, and syncope and
apocope In phonology, apocope () is the omission (elision) or loss of a sound or sounds at the end of a word. While it most commonly refers to the loss of a final vowel, it can also describe the deletion of final consonants or even entire syllables. ...
of vowels. Many of these changes are outlined and exemplified by Hein Steinhauer as follows:


Changes to consonants


Consonant loss

Elision In linguistics, an elision or deletion is the omission of one or more sounds (such as a vowel, a consonant, or a whole syllable) in a word or phrase. However, these terms are also used to refer more narrowly to cases where two words are run to ...
of consonants in all historical positions is extensive throughout Southeast Babar. Ancestral Proto-Malayo-Polynesian sounds like ''*p'', ''*k'', ''*q'', ''*R'', ''*h'' ''*j'' and ''*z'' were simply lost in Southeast Babar with some exceptions. Examples of consonant deletions include: * ''*q'' ** Initially: ''*qapuR'' > ''uir'' "chalk", ''*qatəluR'' > ''kely'' "egg". ** Medially: ''*ma-qitem'' > ''mexm'' "black", ''ma-qudip'' > ''-mory'' "(a)live" ** Finally: ''*tanaq'' > ''kal'' "soil", ''*buaq'' "fruit" > ''wu'', ''*bunuq'' > ''-wuly'' "to kill". * ''*h'' ** ''*hikan'' > ''eːl'' "fish", ''*hapuy'' > ''uy'' "fire". * ''*z'' ** ''*zalan'' > ''al'' "road" * ''*k'' ** Initially: ''*kutu'' > ''oxy'' "louse", ''*kita'' > ''ixy'' "we (inclusive)", ''*kayu'' > ''ay'' "wood", ''*kaRat'' > ''-ax'' "to bite" ** Medially: ''*i-kau'' > ''yow'' "you (sg. polite), ''*hikan'' > ''*ial'' > ''eːl'' "fish" ** Finally: ''*burak'' > ''wo-wor'' "white", ''*tasik'' > ''kat'' "sea", ''*utak'' "brain" > ''ox'' "head" * ''*R'' ** Initially: ''*Rumaq'' > ''em'' "house" ** Medially: ''*kaRat'' > ''-ax'' "to bite", ''*maRi'' > ''-moy'' "to come", ''*daRaq'' > ''ra'' "blood". ** Finally: ''*qatəluR'' > ''kely'' "egg". ** Preserved as ''r'': ''*qapuR'' > ''uir'' "chalk", ''*ma-bəʀ(əq)at'' > ''berk'' "heavy" * ''*p'' ** Initially: ''*pitu'' > ''wo-exy'' "7", ''*punti'' "banana" > ''uty'' ** Medially: ''*hapuy'' > ''uy'' "fire", ''*nipən'' > ''*liəl'' > ''lil'' "tooth" ** Finally: ''ma-qudip'' > ''-mory'' "(a)live", ''*malip'' > ''-moly'' "laugh". ** Preserved as ''p'': ''*panas'' > ''pant'' "warm" * ''*j'' ** ''*qaləjaw'' > ''le'' "day", ''*ŋajan'' > ''non'' "name" ** Preserved as ''r'': ''*ijuŋ'' > ''irl'' "nose"


Chain shift of ''*s'', ''*t'', and ''*k''

The loss of ''*k'' led to a pull
chain shift In historical linguistics, a chain shift is a set of sound changes in which the change in pronunciation of one speech sound (typically, a phoneme) is linked to, and presumably causes, a change in pronunciation of other sounds. The sounds invo ...
. Subsequently, ''*t'' shifted to ''k'' except if the ''*t'' was directly preceded by original ''*n''; unshifted ''*t'' before ''*n'' is known from ''*punti'' > ''uty'' "banana". In turn, ''k'' produced from ''*t''
lenited In linguistics, lenition is a sound change that alters consonants, making them "weaker" in some way. The word ''lenition'' itself means "softening" or "weakening" (from Latin 'weak'). Lenition can happen both synchronically (within a language ...
to /x/ unless the ''*k'' was: * Word- or root-initial and followed by a vowel: ** ''*tanaq'' > ''kal'' "soil", ''*ma-takut'' > ''mkak'' "afraid" (root ''*takut'' "to be afraid"), ''*təlu'' > ''wo-kely'' "3" * Word-final when protected by a consonant that now immediately precedes the ''k'' due to syncope: ** ''*laŋit'' > ''lalk'' "sky, heavens", ''*ma-bəʀ(əq)at'' > ''berk'' "heavy" Lenited reflexes of ''*t'' > ''k'' > ''x'' include ''*teliŋa'' > ''xlil'' "ear", ''*mata'' > ''mox'' "eye", ''*ma-qitəm'' > ''mexm'' "black", ''*matay'' > ''-moxy'' "to die", ''*batu'' > ''waxy'' "stone", and ''*(h)əpat'' > ''wo-ax'' "4". Afterwards, ''*s'' subsequently underwent
fortition In articulatory phonetics, fortition, also known as strengthening, is a consonantal change that increases the degree of stricture. It is the opposite of the more common lenition. For example, a fricative or an approximant may become a stop (i ...
to ''t'', with examples including: * ''*sulu'' > ''tuly'' "torch" * ''*susu'' > ''-tuty'' "milk" * ''*asu'' > ''uty'' "dog". * ''*panas'' > ''pant'' "warm"


Merger of ''*n'', ''*ŋ'' and ''*l''

The inherited Malayo-Polynesian nasal consonants ''*n'' and ''*ŋ'' merge with each other as ''*n'', followed by a merger of that merged phoneme with ''*l'', generally surfacing as /l/. * ''*ta-kaən'' > ''*ka-an'' > ''kaːl'' "you and I eat" * ''*teliŋa'' > ''xlil'' "ear" * ''*nipən'' > ''*lipəl'' > ''*liəl'' > ''lil'' "tooth" * ''*laŋit'' > ''lalk'' "sky, heavens". Post-merger /l/ subsequently underwent an inverse development to ''n'' when adjacent to ''t'' either originating from ''*s'' or borrowed from another language. This circular development leads to roots and inflectional affixes to synchronically contain alternations between ''l'' and ''n''. * English ''bottle'' > Indonesian ''botol'' > Southeast Babar ''potn'' * ''*panas'' > ''*palt'' > ''pant'' "hot" (intermediate ''*l'' preserved in reduplicated intensive derivative ''pal-pant'' "very hot") A few cases of ''n'' failing to merge with ''l'' are known, mainly in monosyllabic words where, due to medial consonant deletion and resulting vowel coalescence, there is simultaneously there is one ''n'' in the onset and a second ''n'' in the coda. * ''*ŋajan'' > ''non'' "name" * ''*na-kaən'' > ''*na-an'' > ''noːn'' "(s)he eats" Due to ''*nipən'' "tooth" surfacing as ''lil'' and not ''**nin'', Steinhauer suggests that the loss of ''*p'' occurred after the loss of ''*j'' and ''*k''.


Other consonants

''*b'' and ''*d'' generally become ''w'' and ''r'', merging with original ''*w'' and ''*r''. * Original ''*w'' and ''*r'': ''*waiR'' > ''wey'' "water", ''*burak'' > ''wo-wor'' "white" * Originally ''*b'' and ''*d'': ''*batu'' > ''waxy'' "stone", ''*babuy'' > ''wawy'' "pig", ''*balik'' > ''-waly'' "to turn", ''*daləm'' > ''ralm'' "inside", ''*dua'' > ''ru'' "2" * Irregularly unchanged ''*b'': ''*ma-bəRat'' > ''berk'' "heavy"


Changes to vowels


Reduction and loss of final vowels

All word-final ''*ə'' and ''*a'', whether inherited as word-final or secondarily word-final due to the loss of a following consonant, are deleted in Southeast Babar. * Originally word-final: ''*lima'' > ''lim'' "hand", ''*mata'' > ''mox'' "eye", ''*dua'' > ''wu-ru'' "2" * Secondarily word-final: ''*tanaq'' > ''*kala'' > ''kal'' "land", ''*dəŋəR'' > ''rel'' "to hear" Word-final high vowels ''*-u'' and ''*-i'' generally reduce to the glide /j/. Like with ''*a'', the loss of a following consonant will make the high vowel count as word-final for the purposes of this reduction. * From ''*u'': ''*batu'' > ''waxy'' "stone", ''*sulu'' > ''tuly'' "torch", ''*baqəRu'' > ''wa-way'' "new" * From ''*i'': ''*malip'' > ''-moly'' "to laugh", ''*balik'' > ''-waly'' "to turn", ''*waiR'' > ''wey'' "water" * From ''*-ay'': ''*matay'' > ''-moxy'' "to die"


Syncope of vowels between two consonants

Vowels often faced deletion in final syllables between two surviving consonants. Examples cited by Steinhauer include ''laŋit'' > ''lalk'' "heavens", ''*daləm'' > ''ralm'' "inside", ''*inum'' > ''*imun'' > ''-iml'' "to drink", ''*panas'' > ''pant'' "warm", and ''*matay'' > ''-moxy'' "to die".


Reflexes of ''*a''

''*a'' turns into ''u'' if it becomes the first phoneme of a word at any point in its evolution to Southeast Babar. * Original word-initial ''*a'': ''*asu'' > ''uty'' "dog", ''*ama'' > ''um'' "father" * Secondarily word-initial ''*a'': ''*hapuy'' > ''uy'' "fire", ''*qabu'' > ''uwy'' "ash" * Exceptions: ''*zalan'' > ''al'' "road" ''*a'' surfaces as ''o'' after nasal consonants. However, this change is blocked in the first-person singular of verbs, where a /j/ is infixed in the verbal root between the nasal and the vowel. Contrast: * ''*ku-malip'' > ''i-myaly'' "I laugh" (rounding blocked by infixed ''-y-'') * ''*na-malip'' > ''l-moly'' "(s)he laughs" (with rounding after nasal). If an ''*a'' is either not word-initial, not preceded by a nasal, or not subject to apocope or syncope, it will remain as ''a''. * Not word initial nor following a nasal: ''*daləm'' > ''ralm'' "inside", ''*babuy'' > ''wawy'' "pig" * Not syncopated nor apocopated: ''*panas'' > ''pant'' "warm"


Reflexes of other vowels

''*u'' remained as ''u'' unless before a syllable containing a non-high vowel, which lowered it to ''o''. The difference in reflexes can be starkly contrasted with ''*buaq'' "fruit", which became the numeral prefix ''wu-'' or ''wo-'' depending on the vowel of the following numeral. * With no lowering: ''wu-ru'' "2", ''wu-lim'' "5" * With lowering: ''wo-kely'' "3", ''wo-ax'' "4", ''wo-lem'' "6" ''*u'' also lowered to ''o'' before /x/, such as in ''*mutaq'' > ''mox'' "to vomit" and ''*kutu'' > ''oxy'' "louse". ''*ə'' is often deleted in various places in trisyllabic words. However, if an ''*ə'' survives these deletions, it is reflected as ''e''. * Deleted: ''*təliŋa'' > ''xlil'' "ear", ''*baqəRu'' > ''wa-way'' "new" * Surfaces as ''e'': ''*dəŋəR'' > ''-rel'' "to hear", ''*ənəm'' > ''-lem'' "6", '' *təlu'' > ''-kely'' "3"


Morphology


Morphophonological processes

Some conditioned sound changes have led to phonetic mutations of morphemes when subject to morphological processes, including affixation and reduplication.


Change of ''l'' to ''n'' before ''t''

Southeast Babar morphemes ending in ''l'' generally change the ''l'' to ''n'' whenever it precedes a ''t''. * ''l-'' (3sg. verbal prefix) + ''-tol'' "to see" + ''tel'' (perfect particle) > ''nton tel'' "(s)he has seen" * ''lewal'' "language" + ''toːl'' "our (inclusive)" > ''lewan toːl'' "our language" * ''-tol'' "ill" > ''-tontol'' "very ill" (reduplicated)


Glide metathesis and insertion

On verbs whose roots end in the glide ''-y'', the glide and the first consonant of the following morpheme undergo metathesis. For instance, ''l-moxy'' "(s)he dies" becomes ''lmox tyel'' "(s)he has died", with the ''y'' from the verb root and the ''t'' of the particle ''tel'' switching places. The metathesis process also occurs when a morpheme ending in ''-y'' is reduplicated, such as ''-kary'' "to work" reduplicating to ''-karkyary'' "to be working". The 1st-person singular, 2nd-person singular, and 2nd-person plural also trigger the insertion of a glide after the first consonant of the following verb stem; the presence of glide insertion is governed by the same restrictions as the application of glide metathesis. Glide metathesis and insertion have some restrictions on their application. * The morpheme the glide might move to or be inserted at cannot have a
high vowel A close vowel, also known as a high vowel (in U.S. terminology), is any in a class of vowel sounds used in many spoken languages. The defining characteristic of a close vowel is that the tongue is positioned approximately as close as possible to ...
after its first consonant. * The initial consonant of the next morpheme cannot be ''w''. If metathesis is blocked by one of these two restrictions, the glide is simply lost unless a vowel follows the glide. If an original Malayo-Polynesian ''*a'' was rounded to ''o'' after a nasal in a base morpheme, the vowel is reverted to ''a'' if the nasal is subject to the glide metathesis or insertion process. The conjugation of ''-moly'' "to laugh" demonstrates this: * Unreduplicated ''l-moly'' "(s)he laughs" and ''i-myaly'' "I laugh" (with glide insertion) * Reduplicated ''l-molmyaly'' "(s)he keeps laughing" and ''i-myalmyaly'' "I keep laughing"


Verb conjugation

Southeast Babar verbs are conjugated for three
grammatical person In linguistics, grammatical person is the grammatical distinction between deictic references to participant(s) in an event; typically, the distinction is between the speaker ( first person), the addressee ( second person), and others ( third p ...
s (first, second, and third persons) and two
grammatical number In linguistics, grammatical number is a Feature (linguistics), feature of nouns, pronouns, adjectives and verb agreement (linguistics), agreement that expresses count distinctions (such as "one", "two" or "three or more"). English and many other ...
s (singular and plural). First-person plural conjugation also distinguishes
clusivity In linguistics, clusivity is a grammatical distinction between ''inclusive'' and ''exclusive'' first-person pronouns and verbal morphology, also called ''inclusive " we"'' and ''exclusive "we"''. Inclusive "we" specifically includes the address ...
, with exclusive 1st-person plural excluding the addressee but the inclusive 1st-person plural including them. In addition, verbs can additionally inflect for at least three known
grammatical aspect In linguistics, aspect is a grammatical category that expresses how a verbal action, event, or state, extends over time. For instance, perfective aspect is used in referring to an event conceived as bounded and unitary, without reference t ...
s, namely
perfect Perfect commonly refers to: * Perfection; completeness, and excellence * Perfect (grammar), a grammatical category in some languages Perfect may also refer to: Film and television * ''Perfect'' (1985 film), a romantic drama * ''Perfect'' (20 ...
,
progressive aspect The continuous and progressive aspects (abbreviated and ) are grammatical aspects that express incomplete action ("to do") or state ("to be") in progress at a specific time: they are non-habitual, imperfective aspects. In the grammars of many l ...
, and
inchoative aspect Inchoative aspect (list of glossing abbreviations, abbreviated or ), also known as inceptive, is a grammatical aspect, referring to the beginning of a state. It can be found in conservative Indo-European languages such as Latin and Lithuanian lang ...
.


Person-number prefixes

Southeast Babar conjugates verbs for person and number via a series of prefixes attached to verb stems, either to an unmarked stem or the progressive stem. There are two basic classes of person-number prefixes in the language. One class has the person-number prefixes all contain a vowel, and the other class where most of the prefixes do not. The prefixes for each class are as follows:


Verbal aspect marking

A particle ''tel'' follows the conjugated verb to mark the
perfect Perfect commonly refers to: * Perfection; completeness, and excellence * Perfect (grammar), a grammatical category in some languages Perfect may also refer to: Film and television * ''Perfect'' (1985 film), a romantic drama * ''Perfect'' (20 ...
aspect. Another postverbal particle ''kay'' marks the
inchoative Inchoative aspect (abbreviated or ), also known as inceptive, is a grammatical aspect, referring to the beginning of a state. It can be found in conservative Indo-European languages such as Latin and Lithuanian, and also in Finnic languages or Eu ...
aspect. The two particles can stack together to form an "inchoative perfect", so to speak. Steinbauer demonstrates the two particles with the following examples. * ''lmoxy'' "(s)he dies" * ''lmox tyel'' "(s)he has died" * ''lmok kyay tyel'' "(s)he has already died"
Progressive aspect The continuous and progressive aspects (abbreviated and ) are grammatical aspects that express incomplete action ("to do") or state ("to be") in progress at a specific time: they are non-habitual, imperfective aspects. In the grammars of many l ...
is expressed by a special verbal stem for each verb formed via
reduplication In linguistics, reduplication is a Morphology (linguistics), morphological process in which the Root (linguistics), root or Stem (linguistics), stem of a word, part of that, or the whole word is repeated exactly or with a slight change. The cla ...
of the verb root. * ''lkary'' "(s)he works" > ''lkarkyary'' "(s)he is working" * ''lmoly'' "(s)he laughs" > ''lmolmyaly'' "(s)he keeps laughing". * Proto-Malayo-Polynesian ''*kaən'' > reduplicated ''*kakan'' > ''-kkol'' "is eating" (present ''-o:l'' ~ ''-a:l-'')


Pronouns

The basic personal pronouns in Southeast Babar are:


Possessive markers

There are also at least two series of possessive markers, one series ending in ''u'' and another one ending in ''oːl''. They all are stressed when attached to a possessed noun. Possessive markers documented by Steinhauer are first-person singular ''u'' and ''oːl'', second-person singular ''mu'' and ''moːl'', and first-person inclusive plural ''toːl''. Possessive markers follow the noun that is possessed. Examples of this behaviour include: * ''u''-series: ''ox u'' "my head", ''ox mu'' "your (sg.) head" * ''oːl''-series: ''lim oːl'' "my hand", ''lim moːl'' "your (sg.) hand", ''lewan toːl'' "our (incl.) language"


Numerals

Cardinal numerals from 2 to 9 have an obligatory prefix ''wu-''/''wo-'' that is derived from ''*buaq'' "fruit", which on the way to Southeast Babar evolved into a classifier and then a prefix. The cardinals from 2-7 are given by Steinhauer as ''wu-ru'' "2", ''wo-kely'' "3", ''wo-ax'' "4", ''wu-lim'' "5", ''wo-lem'' "6", and ''wo-exy'' "7". Other numerals are only recorded in Taber's wordlist, including ''metl'' "1", ''wo-ka'' "8", ''wu-si'' "9", and ''wu-ki'' "10".


References

{{Languages of Indonesia Babar languages Languages of the Maluku Islands