History
The two main analyses of small clauses originate with Edwin Williams (1975, 1980) and Tim Stowell (1981). Williams' analysis follows the Theory of Predication, where the "subject" is the "external argument of a maximal projection". In contrast, Stowell's theory follows the Theory of Small Clauses, supported by linguists such as Chomsky, Aarts, and Kitagawa. This theory uses X-bar theory to treat small clauses as constituents. Linguists debate which analysis to pursue, as there is evidence for both sides of the debate.Williams (1975, 1980)
The term "small clause" was coined by Edwin Williams in 1975, who specifically looked at "reduced relatives, adverbial modifier phrases, and gerundive phrases". The following three examples are treated in Williams' 1975 paper as "small clauses", as cited in Balazs 2012. However, not all linguists consider these to be small clauses according to the term's modern definition. # ''The man'' ''driving the bus'' ''is Norton's best friend.'' # ''John decided to leave,'' ''thinking the party was over'' # ''John’s evading his taxes'' ''infuriates me''. The modern definition of a small clause is an P XPin a predicative relationship. This definition was proposed by Edwin Williams in 1980, who introduced the concept of Predication. He proposed that the subject NP and the predicate XP are related via co-indexation, which is made possible by c-command. In Williams' analysis, the P XPof a small clause does not form a constituent.Stowell (1981)
Timothy Stowell in 1981 analyzed the small clause as a constituent, pg. 87-88. and proposed a structure using X-bar theory. Stowell proposes that the subject is defined as an NP occurring in a specifier position, that case is assigned in the specifier position, and that not all categories have subjects. His analysis explains why case-marked subjects cannot occur in infinitival clauses, although NPs can be projected up to an infinitival clause's specifier position. Stowell considers the following examples to be small clauses and constituents. #Contexts
What does and does not qualify as a small clause varies in the literature: the example sentences in (8) contain (what some theories of syntax judge to be) small clauses. In each example, the posited small clause is in boldface, and the underlined expression functions as a predicate over the nominal immediately to its left, which is the subject. The verbs that license small clauses are a heterogeneous set, and fall into five classes: * ''raising-to-object'' or '' ECM'' verbs like ''consider'' and ''want'' in (8a); these were the focus of early discussions of small clauses * verbs like ''call'' and ''name'', which subcategorize for an object NP and aStructural analyses
Broadly speaking, there are three competing analyses of the structure of small clauses. * the flat structure analysis treats the subject and predicate of the small clause as sister constituents * the layered structure analysis treats the subject and predicate as a single "small clause" (SC) constituent * the X-bar theory analysis treats the subject and predicate as a single constituent projected from the head of the small clause, which may be V, N, A, or P (with some analyses having additional functional structure)Flat structure
The flat structure organizes small clause material into two distinct sister constituents. The a-trees on the left are the phrase structure trees, and the b-trees on the right are the dependency trees. The key aspect of these structures is that the small clause material consists of two separate sister constituents. The flat analysis is preferred by those working in dependency grammars and representational phrase structure grammars (e.g.Layered structure
The layered structure organizes small clause material into one constituent. The phrase structure trees are again on the left, and the dependency trees on the right. To mark the small clause in the phrase structure trees, the node label SC is used. The layered analysis is preferred by those working in theX-Bar Theory structures
''SeeAnalysis 1: symmetric constituent
In this analysis, neither of the constituents determine the category, meaning that it is an exocentric construction. Some linguists believe that the label of this structure can be symmetrically determined by the constituents, and others believe that this structure lacks a label altogether.Moro, Andrea. (2008). The anomaly of copular sentences. ''Unpublished manuscript'', ''University of Venice.'' In order to indicate a predicative relationship between the subject (in this case, the NP Mary), and the predicate (AP smart), some have suggested a system of co-indexation, where the subject mustAnalysis 2: projection of the predicate
In this analysis, the small clause can be identified as a projection of the predicate (in this example, the predicate would be the 'smart' in 'Mary smart'). In this view, the specifier of the structure (in this case, the NP 'Mary') is the subject of the head (in this case, the A 'smart'). This analysis builds on Chomsky's model of phrase structure and is proposed by Stowell and Contreras.Analysis 3: projection of a functional category
The PrPBowers, J. (1993). The Syntax of Predication. ''Linguistic Inquiry,24''(4), pg. 596-597. Retrieved from (predicate phrase) category (also analyzed as AgrP, PredP, and PBalazs, Julie E. (2012). The Syntax of Small Clauses. pg. 23. ''Masters Thesis, Cornell University.''), was proposed for a few reasons, some of which are outlined below: * This structure helps to account forIdentification tests
A small clause divides into two constituents: the subject and its predicate. While small clauses occur cross-linguistically, different languages have different restrictions on what can and cannot be a well-formed (i.e.,Absence of tense-marking
A small clause is characterised as having two constituents NP and XP that enter into a predicative relation, but lacking finite tense and/or a verb. Possible predicates in small clauses typically include adjective phrases (AP), prepositional phrases (PPs), noun phrases (NPs), or determiner phrases (DPs) (seeSelectional restrictions
Selected by matrix verb
Small clauses satisfy selectional requirements of the verb in the main clause in order to be grammatical. The argument structure of verbs is satisfied with small clause constructions. The following two examples show how the argument structure of the verb "consider" affects what predicate can be in the small clause. #Semantically determined
Small clauses' grammaticality judgments are affected by their semantic value. The following examples show how semantic selection also affects predication of a small clause. #Negation
Small clauses may not be negated by a negative modal or auxiliary verb, such as ''don't, shan't'', or ''can't''. Small clauses may only be negated by negative particles, such as ''not''. #Constituency
There are a number of considerations that support or refute the one or the other analysis. The layered analysis, which, again, views the small clause as a constituent, is supported by the basic insight that the small clause functions as a single semantic unit, i.e. as a clause consisting of a subject and a predicate.Coordination
Only constituents of a like type can be joined via coordination. Small clauses can be coordinated, which suggests they are constituents of a like type, but seeSubjecthood
The layered analysis is also supported by the fact that in certain cases, a small clause can function as the subject of the greater clause, e.g. #Complement of ''with''
Further, small clauses can appear as the complement of ''with'', e.g.: #Movement
One could argue, however, that small clauses in subject position and as the complement of ''with'' are fundamentally different from small clauses in object position. Some datapoints have the small clause following the matrix verb, whereby the subject of the small clause is also the object of the matrix clause. In such cases, the matrix verb appears to be subcategorizing for its object noun (phrase), which then functions as the subject of the small clause. In this regard, there are a number of observations suggesting that the object/subject noun phrase is a direct dependent of the matrix verb. If so, then this means the flat structure is the correct analysis. This captures that fact, with such object/subject noun phrases, as illustrated in (47), the small clause generally does not behave as a single constituent with respect to movement diagnostics. Thus, the "subject" of a small clause cannot participate in topicalization (47b), clefting (47c), pseudo-cleating (47d), nor can it served as an answer fragment (47e). Moreover, like ordinary object NPs, the "subject" of a small clause can becomes the subject of the corresponding passive sentence (47f), and can be realized as a reflexive pronoun that is coindexed with the matrix subject (47g). The datapoints in (47b-g) are consistent with the flat analysis of small clauses: in such an analysis the object of the matrix clause plays a dual role insofar as it is also the subject of the embedded predicate.Counter-Arguments
Small clauses' constituency status is not agreed upon by linguists. Some linguists argue that small clauses do not form a constituent, but rather form a noun phrase. One argument is that P AP smallclauses cannot occur in the subject position without modification, as shown by the ungrammatically of (48). However, these P APsmall clauses can occur after the verb if they are modified, such as in example (49). #Cross-linguistic variation
Raising-to-object
Complement small clauses are related to the phenomena of raising-to-object, therefore this theory will be discussed in more detail for English and Korean.English
Raising-to-object with a direct object is illustrated in (52) with the verb ''proved.'' The bolded constituents represent the small clause of the sentence. By hypothesis, the raising-to-object analysis treats the subject of the small clause as having raised from the embedded small clause to the main clause ''''Korean
In Korean, raising-to-object is optional from with complement clauses, but obligatory with complement small clauses. A fully inflected complement clause is given in (55), and the object ''Mary'' can be marked either with nominative case (55a) or with accusative case (55b). In contrast, with a complement small clause as in (56), the subject of the small clause can only be marked with accusative; thus while (56a) is ill-formed, (56b) is well-formed.Categorical restrictions
French (Romance)
At first glance, French small clauses appear to be unrestricted relative to which category can realize a small clause. Illustrative examples are given below: there are P APsmall clauses (57); P PPsmall clauses (58), as well as P VPsmall clauses (59). However, there are some restrictions on NP VP constructions. The verb in example (59) is infinitival, without inflected tense, and takes a PP complement. However, the following example (d) is an NP VP small clause construction that is ungrammatical. Although the verb here is infinitival, it cannot grammatically take an AP complement. Coordination tests in French do not provide consistent evidence for small clauses' constituency. Below is an example (e) proving small clauses' constituency. The two small clauses in this example use an NP AP construction. However, the example (f) below makes an incorrect prediction about constituency. Sportiche provides two possible interpretations of this data: either coordination is not a reliable constituency test or the current theory of constituency should be revised to include strings such as the ones predicted above.Lithuanian (Balto-Slavic)
Lithuanian small clauses may occur in a NP NP or NP AP construction. NP PP constructions are not small clauses in Lithuanian as the PP does not enter into a predicative relationship with the NP. The example (a) below is of an NP NP construction. The example (b) below is of an NP AP construction. While the English translation of the sentence includes the auxiliary verb "was", it is not present in Lithuanian. In Lithuanian, small clauses may be moved to the front of the sentence to become the topic. This suggests that the small clause operates as a single unit, or a constituent. Note that the sentence in example (c) in English is ungrammatical so it is marked with an asterisk, but the sentence is grammatical in Lithuanian. The phrase ''her an immature brat'' cannot be split up in example (d), which provides further evidence that the small clause behaves as a single unit.Mandarin (Sinitic)
In Mandarin, a small clause does not only lack a verb and tense, but also the presence of functional projections. The reason for this is that the lexical entries for particular nouns in Mandarin not only contain the categorical feature for nouns, but also for verbs. Thus even with the lack of functional projections, nominals can be predicative in a small clause. (a) illustrates a complement small clause: it has no tense-marking, only a DP subject and an NP predicate. However, the semantic difference between Mandarin Chinese and English with regards to its small clauses are represented by example (b) and (c). Though (b) is the embedded small clause in the previous example, it cannot be a matrix clause. Despite having the same sentence structure, a small clause consisting of a DP and an NP, due to the ability of a nominal expression to also belong to a second category of verbs, example (c) is a grammatical sentence. This is evidence that there are more restrictive constraints on what is considered a small clause in Mandarin Chinese, which requires further research. Below is case of special usage of small clause used with the possessive verb ''yǒu''. The small clause is underlined. Here, the possessive verb ''yǒu'' takes a small clause complement in order to make a degree comparison between the subject and indirect object. Due to the following AP ''gāo'', here the possessive verb ''yǒu'' expresses a limit of the degree of tallness. It is only with a small clause complement that this uncommon degree use of the possessive verb can be communicated.Variable constituent order
Brazilian Portuguese
In Brazilian Portuguese, there are two types of small clauses: free small clauses and dependent small clauses. Dependent small clauses are similar to English in that they consist of an NP XP in a predicative relation. Like many other Romance languages, Brazilian Portuguese has free subject-predicate inversion, although it is restricted here to verbs with single arguments. Dependent small clauses may appear in either a standard, as in example (a), or an inverted form, as in example (b). In contrast, free small clauses cannot occur with subject-predicate order: in example (c), using an P APorder renders the sentence. Free small clauses only occur in the inverted form: in example (d) the small clause has an P NPorder, specifically an P NPorder. The classification of free small clauses is under debate. Some linguists argue that these free small clauses are actually cleft sentences with finite tense, while other linguists believe that free small clauses are tense phrases without inflected tense on the surface.Spanish
In Spanish, like many Romance languages, there is some flexibility in small clause construction due to the flexibility in word order. This is posited to be due to the fact that Spanish is an example of a language that is discourse-prominent and agreement-oriented. This passing of features onto the v allows a separation of the object from the verb when the focus of the sentence changes. The final position in a sentence is reserved for the focus as seen by the differences in (a) and (b). The difference in preference for one construction over the other ( P NPversus P XP is determined by discourse features. Refer to the following two examples. In (c) the establish topic is the XP, AP in this case, meaning the information we are seeking is the NP. Answer In the following example (e) the reverse is true. We are given the NP in the question and are seeking the information of the XP. Answer Notice in (d) and (f) that the English answer remains the same regardless of the question, but in Spanish, one ordering is preferred over the other. When the new information being presented is the XP, the construction preferred is P XP This is because the sentence-final position is reserved for focus. It is worth noting that the non-preferred formations (d)(ii) and (f)(i) can be accepted as grammatical if the new information is given the prosodic stress or the established information is destressed, and there is a longer pause between the two constituents, making it right-dislocated.Greek
Greek is another example of a language that is discourse-prominent and agreement-oriented, allowing features to be passed onto the v. This allows for flexibility in word order depending on the changing focus of the small clause. This example can be shown in (a) and (b). The construction can either take P NPor P XPformations with the focused constituent appearing sentence-finally. The difference in preference for one construction over the other is determined by discourse features. Newly given information is considered the focus of the sentence and is therefore preferred in sentence-final position. Refer to examples (c) and (e). In (c) the information we are given is the XP (AP in this case) and the information we are seeking is the DP. This means that the preferred construction is P DP The reverse is true of example (e). Answer Answer It is worth noting that the non-preferred formations (d)(ii) and (f)(i) can be accepted as grammatical if the new information not in sentence-final position is given the emphatic stress.Expressive exclamatives
English
Expressive Small Clauses, like SCs are verbless and the noun does not carry descriptive content but instead carries expressive content.Izumi, Y., & Hayashi, S. (2018). Expressive small clauses in japanese. (pp. 188-199). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-93794-6_13 Expressive Small Clauses are evidence that small clauses learned in early development, last until adulthood for language speakers.Citko, B. (2011). Small clauses. ''Language and Linguistics Compass,'' 5(10), 748-763. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2011.00312.x ESCs are illustrated in (a). Expressive small clauses are never used in an argument position of the phrase as seen in (b-i) and do not generally occur within the embedded clause of a sentence as seen in (b-ii). Both of the examples below are ungrammatical. The bolded constituents are the ESCs. Unlike ESCs in English, Japanese ESCs differ in two ways: second person pronouns are not used, and ESCs sometimes appear in argument position. The example below shows a well-formed ESC in Japanese.Japanese
The phrase in (a) illustrates the pattern found in Japanese ESCs: 1''—no—''NP2">P1''—no—''NP2 (a) illustrates the use of a proximateInformation structure
English: intonation
Because English is agreement-prominent, there is inflexible SC word order and a heavy importance on intonational focus. Though both answers in English use the same words, focus is given by prosodic stress.Spanish: word order and intonation
Spanish has a flexible SC word order, and word order determines focus but prosodic stress is able to be used to make non-preferred constructions felicitous. These examples show the non-felicitous construction but they would be accepted by speakers if the underlined constituents are given emphatic stress and precede a long pause.See also
*References
Literature
*Aarts, B. 1992. Small clauses in English: the non-verbal types. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. *Borsley, R. 1991. Syntactic theory: A unified approach. London: Edward Arnold. *Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on government and binding: The Pisa lectures. Berlin:Mouton de Gruyter. *Chomsky, N. 1986. Barriers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. *Culicover, P. 1997. Principles and parameters: An introduction to syntactic theory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. *Culicover, P. and R. Jackendoff. 2005. Simpler syntax. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. *Haegeman, L. 1994. Introduction to government and binding theory, 2nd edition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. *Haegeman, L. and J. Guéron 1999. English grammar: A generative perspective. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers. *Matthews, P. 2007. Syntactic relations: A critical survey. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. *Ouhalla, J. 1994. Transformational grammar: From rules to principles and parameters. London: Edward Arnold. *Wardhaugh, R. 2003. Understanding English grammar, second edition. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. {{refend Clauses