Roughhousing
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Rough-and-tumble play, also called play fighting, is a form of
play Play most commonly refers to: * Play (activity), an activity done for enjoyment * Play (theatre), a work of drama Play may refer also to: Computers and technology * Google Play, a digital content service * Play Framework, a Java framework * P ...
where participants compete with one another attempting to obtain certain advantages (such as
biting Biting is an action involving a set of teeth closing down on an object. It is a common zoological behavior, being found in toothed animals such as mammals, reptiles, amphibians, fishes, and arthropods. Biting is also an action humans participate ...
or pushing the opponent onto the ground) but play in this way without the severity of genuine
fighting Combat ( French for ''fight'') is a purposeful violent conflict between multiple combatants with the intent to harm the opposition. Combat may be armed (using weapons) or unarmed ( not using weapons). Combat is resorted to either as a method of ...
(which rough-and-tumble play resembles). Rough-and-tumble play is one of the most common forms of play in both
humans Humans (''Homo sapiens'') or modern humans are the most common and widespread species of primate, and the last surviving species of the genus ''Homo''. They are Hominidae, great apes characterized by their Prehistory of nakedness and clothing ...
and non-human
animals Animals are multicellular, eukaryotic organisms in the biological kingdom Animalia (). With few exceptions, animals consume organic material, breathe oxygen, have myocytes and are able to move, can reproduce sexually, and grow from a ...
. It has been pointed out that despite its apparent
aggressiveness Aggression is behavior aimed at opposing or attacking something or someone. Though often done with the intent to cause harm, some might channel it into creative and practical outlets. It may occur either reactively or without provocation. In h ...
, rough-and-tumble play is helpful for encouraging cooperative behavior and cultivation of
social skills A social skill is any competence facilitating interaction and communication with others where social rules and relations are created, communicated, and changed in verbal and nonverbal ways. The process of learning these skills is called socia ...
. For rough-and-tumble play to remain "play" (instead of spiraling into a real fight), there has to be cooperation (e.g., with participants agreeing to not actually exert forces in pretend punches). Sometimes, one participant may push or hit harder than expected, and then the other participants will have to decide whether it was an unintended mistake or a malicious transgression. Thus, rough-and-tumble play involves considerable social reasoning and judgment.


Sexual Dimorphism

This form of play exhibits notable sexual dimorphism in many mammalian species, including humans. Males typically engage in this type of play more frequently and intensively than females, a pattern observed across diverse taxa ranging from rodents to primates. This dimorphism is thought to result from the influence of prenatal and early postnatal hormones, particularly androgens like testosterone, which shape the neural circuits governing social and play behavior. Researchers propose that these sex differences may have evolutionary significance, contributing to the development of motor skills, dominance behaviors, and social strategies that were advantageous for reproductive success in ancestral environments.


Mammals


Carnivora

Male carnivores tend to have higher rates of rough-and-tumble play compared to females. This is potentially due to the fact that males are typically larger, due to
polygamous Polygamy (from Late Greek , "state of marriage to many spouses") is the practice of marrying multiple spouses. When a man is married to more than one wife at the same time, it is called polygyny. When a woman is married to more than one h ...
or
promiscuous Promiscuity is the practice of engaging in sexual activity frequently with different partners or being indiscriminate in the choice of sexual partners. The term can carry a moral judgment. A common example of behavior viewed as promiscuous by man ...
mating systems, as well as energy requirements imposed on females by feeding young. Conflicting results have arisen from
Pinnipeds Pinnipeds (pronounced ), commonly known as seals, are a widely distributed and diverse clade of carnivorous, fin-footed, semiaquatic, mostly marine mammals. They comprise the extant families Odobenidae (whose only living member is the walr ...
. Some studies report that male and female pups do not show significant differences in engagement in rough-and-tumble play, while others suggest that male pups spend more time engaged in dyadic play bouts. Male Galapagos fur seal pups (''Arctocephalus galapagoensis'') are reported to engage in play bouts that are twice as long as those seen in female conspecifics. Rough-and-tumble play has also been observed in South American fur seals (''Arctocephalus australis'') and harbour seals (''Phoca vitulina''). Rough-and-tumble play has been reported in the following canine species: domestic dogs (''Canis familiaris''), coyotes (''Canis latrans''), wolves (''Canis lupus occidentalis''), bush dogs (''Speothos venaticus''), crab-eating foxes (''Cerdocyon thous''), and maned wolves (''Chrysocyon brachyurus''). In domestic puppies, males initiate more often than females and will initiate more often in mixed-sex dyads. No sexed differences have been reported in wolves, bush dogs, crab-eating foxes or maned wolves. In Felidae, rough-and-tumble play has been reported in the Eurasian lynx (''Lynx lynx''), Far-Eastern wild cat (''Prionailurus bengalensis euptilura''), and domestic cat (''Felis catus''), though no known sexed differences occur between male and female kittens. Meerkats (''Suricata suricatta'') also show no sexual dimorphism in play frequencies or initiations.


Primates

Primates are known for their relatively long juvenile development periods, which are often associated with juvenile play. Long juvenile periods, small litter sizes, long intervals between births, extended lifespans and high investment in offsprings mean that primates are often an ideal model for looking at play behaviour. In the ring-tailed lemur (''Lemur catta''), female infants are found to engage in social play more frequently than male conspecifics, although there are some conflicting reports. In the family Atelidae, black-handed spider monkeys (''Ateles geofroyi'') are reported to have male infants are play more frequently than females and are more likely to engage in play bouts. Comparatively, in the Yucatán black howler monkey (''Alouatta pigra''), there are no known sexed differences. In the mantled howler monkey (''Alouatta palliata''), males have been reported to play less. In the family Callitrichidae, males are reported to play more than female common marmosets (''Callithrix jacchus''). In contrast, female saddle-backed tamarins (''Saguinus fuscicollis'') are reported to play more often than males. In the Cebidae family, male tufted capuchins (''Cebus apella'') spend more time wrestling and chasing than females. This pattern is seen again in the squirrel monkey (''Saimiri sciureus''). Captive coppery titi monkeys (''Callicebus cupreus'') have not shown any sex-related differences in juvenile rough-and-tumble play. Male Japanese macaques (''Macaca fuscata'') play and initiate more frequently than female conspecifics. This pattern is reported again in the rhesus macaque (''Macaca mulata''), and that males also had higher frequencies of success in play bouts. These patterns are also reported in the stumptail (''Macaca arctoides'') and crested (''Macaca nigra'') macaques. Juvenile Yellow baboons (''Papio cynocephalus'') appear to spend the same amount of time rough-and-tumble playing, regardless of sex. In the Hamadryas baboon (''Papio hamadryas''), males spend more time engaged in dyadic play. This is seen again in the Olive baboon (''Papio anubis''), where males also engage more frequently in mouth-and-wrestle play. Geladas (''Theropithecus gelada'') show no sexed differences in rough-and-tumble play, although some reports suggest that in the wild, males play slightly more often than females. Vervets (''Cercopithecus aethiops'') have juvenile males that engage more frequently and more aggressively in rough-and-tumble play. It is suggested however that these patterns are influenced heavily by age in this species, and that females engage in more social play later in their development (48 to 61 months). Multiple guenon species are known to show rough-and-tumble play, with a male bias. This includes: the samango monkey (''Cercopithecus mitis erythrarchus''), the talapoin (''Miopithecus talapoin''), patas (''Erythrocebus patas''), the redtail monkey (''Cercopithecus ascanius'') and the blue monkey (''Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni''). Male sooty mangabeys (''Cercocebus atys'') are reported to play more often than females. In contrast, there are no reported sex differences in play in immature grey-cheeked mangabeys (''Lophocebus albigena'').Lucci SG, Rothman, JM. "Rough-and-tumble play in immature monkeys at Kibale National Park, Uganda." p. 167. In: Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (''Rhinopithecus roxellana'') have a male bias towards rough-and-tumble play. There is no reported bias in the similarly related species the black-and-white colobus monkeys (''Colobus guereza'') or red colobus monkeys (''Procolobus rufomitratus''). In chimpanzees (''Pan troglodytes''), males are known to engage more frequently in play than females. Age, setting, components of play and age group are all known factors that affect how long and how often immature chimpanzees engage in rough-and-tumble play.


Rodents

Rough-and-tumble play has been observed in captive prairie voles (''Microtus ochrogaster'') and wild capybara (''Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris''), irrespective of sex. In wild yellow-bellied marmosets (''Marmota flaviventris''), male yearlings participate in bouts more frequently than female conspecifics. In Columbian ground squirrels (''Spermophilus columbianus''), reports vary, with some suggesting that males engage and participate in rough-and-tumble play more frequently, while others suggest there is no significant observed sex bias. Belding's ground squirrels (''Urocitellus beldingi'') also engage in juvenile rough-and-tumble play. Results from captive golden hamsters (''Mesocricetus auratus'') suggest that males engage more frequently in bouts and longer than females. This pattern is also observed in the juvenile rough-and-tumble play behavior of captive hooded rats (''Rattus norvegicus'').


Cetaceans (Whales and dolphins)

Low levels of social play have been recorded in both sexes of bottlenose dolphin (''Tursiops truncatus'') calves. Rough-and-tumble play has been observed in belugas (''Delphinapterus leucas''), in both juveniles and adults.


Chiroptera (Bats)

Rough-and-tumble play has been observed in the common vampire bat (''Desmodus rotundus''), with a male bias towards initiation.


Dasyuromorphia (Carnivorous marsupials)

In captive kowaris (''Dasyuroides byrnie''), rough-and-tumble play has been observed in both sexes of juveniles. Similarly, juvenile rough-and-tumble play has been reported in both sexes of spotted-tail quolls (''Dasyurus maculatus'').


Diprotodontia (Marsupials)

In captive, red-necked wallabies (''Macropus rufogriseus banksianus'') rough-and-tumble play is reported. This play behaviour is more common in males, with juvenile females rarely observed engaging. Playfighting has been reported in the red kangaroo (''Macropus rufus'') between mother and offspring.


Perissodactyla (Odd-toed ungulates)

Multiple studies have investigated rough-and-tumble play in horses (''Equus caballus''). One study reports that male Jeju pony foals are more likely to engage in this kind of play after grooming compared to female conspecifics. Another study reports that juvenile male Icelandic horses engage in rough-and-tumble play more frequently than juvenile females. This pattern has also been observed in Welsh ponies, Camargue's and feral horses.


Artiodactyla (Even-toed ungulates)

Play-fighting has been reported in adult and juvenile male goitered gazelles (''Gazella subgutturosa''). Similarly, it is also reported in juvenile pronghorns ''(Antilocapra americana)'' and guanacos ''(Lama guanicoe).''


Proboscidea (Elephants)

Rough-and-tumble play is observed in both African (''Loxodonta africana'') and Asian elephant (''Elephas maximus'') calves. It has been noted that there is an increase in male bias in captive animals compared to wild.


References

{{reflist


External links


Rough and Tumble Play
''Scholarpedia''. Play (activity) Aggression Ethology Learning Childhood Mock combat Fight play