Within
linguistics
Linguistics is the scientific study of language. The areas of linguistic analysis are syntax (rules governing the structure of sentences), semantics (meaning), Morphology (linguistics), morphology (structure of words), phonetics (speech sounds ...
, obviative (
abbreviated ) third person is a
grammatical-person marking that distinguishes a
referent
A referent ( ) is a person or thing to which a name – a linguistic expression or other symbol – refers. For example, in the sentence ''Mary saw me'', the referent of the word ''Mary'' is the particular person called Mary who is being spoken o ...
that is less important to the discourse from one that is more important (proximate). The obviative is sometimes referred to as the "fourth person".
Comparison with other grammatical-person marking systems
In
English and many other
European languages
There are over 250 languages indigenous to Europe, and most belong to the Indo-European language family. Out of a total European population of 744 million as of 2018, some 94% are native speakers of an Indo-European language. The three larges ...
, the principal means of distinguishing between multiple third-person referents is using gender or (lack of) reflexive. Thus, in "she saw him", it is clear that there are two third persons because they are of different genders. In "she saw her", it is clear that there are two third persons because otherwise, one would say "she saw herself". However, "she saw her dog" is ambiguous: it could mean that she saw her own dog or that she saw someone else's dog.
This is because it is not clear, in some contexts, if "she" and "her" refer to the same person.
An obviative/proximate system has a different way of distinguishing between multiple third-person referents. When there is more than one third person named in a sentence or discourse context, the most important, salient, or topical is marked as "proximate" and any other, less salient entities are marked as "obviative". Subsequent sentences that refer to previously-named entities with pronouns or verbal inflections can then use the proximate and obviative references that have already been established to distinguish between the two.
For example, in the sentence "
the quick brown fox jumps over the lazy dog
"The quick brown fox jumps over the lazy dog" is an English-language pangram a sentence (linguistics), sentence that contains all the letters of the alphabet. The phrase is commonly used for Touch typing, touch-typing practice, testing typewrit ...
", there are two third-person referents, the fox and the dog. Thus, one of them has to be proximate and the other one has to be obviative, depending on which one the speaker considers more central to the story. If the fox is the more important one, the sentence might look something like "the quick brown fox- jumps- the lazy dog-", where is verbal inflection indicating a proximate subject acting on an obviative object. In that case, a subsequent sentence "and then went- away" would mean that the ''fox'' went away.
On the other hand, if the dog is the more important one, the sentence might look something like "the quick brown fox- jumps- the lazy dog-", where is verbal inflection indicating an obviative subject acting on a proximate object. In that case, the same subsequent sentence "and then went- away" would mean instead that the ''dog'' went away. By contrast, an equivalent subsequent sentence in English, such as "and then he went away", would not necessarily indicate whether "he" is the fox or the dog.
An analogy that has been used to explain obviation is that the proximate is the entity in the "spotlight", and any other, obviative entities are out of the spotlight or "hangers-on".
Geography
North America
Obviate/proximate distinctions are common in some indigenous language families in northern
North America
North America is a continent in the Northern Hemisphere, Northern and Western Hemisphere, Western hemispheres. North America is bordered to the north by the Arctic Ocean, to the east by the Atlantic Ocean, to the southeast by South Ameri ...
.
Algonquian languages
The Algonquian languages ( ; also Algonkian) are a family of Indigenous languages of the Americas and most of the languages in the Algic language family are included in the group. The name of the Algonquian language family is distinguished from ...
are perhaps best known for obviation, but the feature occurs also in some
Salishan languages
The Salishan languages ( ), also known as the Salish languages ( ), are a Language family, family of languages found in the Pacific Northwest in North America, namely the Canadian province of British Columbia and the American states of Washingt ...
and in the
language isolate
A language isolate is a language that has no demonstrable genetic relationship with any other languages. Basque in Europe, Ainu and Burushaski in Asia, Sandawe in Africa, Haida and Zuni in North America, Kanoê in South America, and Tiwi ...
Kutenai as well as in the more southern
Keresan languages.
Africa
Obviative markers are used in
Africa
Africa is the world's second-largest and second-most populous continent after Asia. At about 30.3 million km2 (11.7 million square miles) including adjacent islands, it covers 20% of Earth's land area and 6% of its total surfac ...
in some
Nilo-Saharan
The Nilo-Saharan languages are a proposed family of around 210 African languages spoken by somewhere around 70 million speakers, mainly in the upper parts of the Chari and Nile rivers, including historic Nubia, north of where the two tributari ...
and
Niger–Congo languages.
Eurasia
Obviation has also been attested in the
Northeast Caucasian Ingush language in
Asia
Asia ( , ) is the largest continent in the world by both land area and population. It covers an area of more than 44 million square kilometres, about 30% of Earth's total land area and 8% of Earth's total surface area. The continent, which ...
.
Cross-linguistic patterns
*If animacy is involved, animate
noun phrase
A noun phrase – or NP or nominal (phrase) – is a phrase that usually has a noun or pronoun as its head, and has the same grammatical functions as a noun. Noun phrases are very common cross-linguistically, and they may be the most frequently ...
s tend to be proximate, and inanimate
noun phrases tend to be obviative.
*Possessors are frequently required to be proximate, with possessees thus required to be obviative.
*Obviation is most common in
head-marking languages since the obviative is useful in disambiguating otherwise unmarked nominals.
*The obviative referent seems to be always the marked form, and the proximate is unmarked.
*Obviative marking tends to apply only to the third person, but it has been attested in the second person in a handful of
Nilo-Saharan languages
The Nilo-Saharan languages are a proposed family of around 210 African languages spoken by somewhere around 70 million speakers, mainly in the upper parts of the Chari River, Chari and Nile rivers, including historic Nubia, north of where the tw ...
.
*Proximate/obviative assignments are preserved throughout the clauses and are also often constant over longer discourse segments.
Examples
Ojibwe
The following is a typical example of obviate/proximate morphology in the Eastern dialect of the Algonquian
Ojibwe
The Ojibwe (; Ojibwe writing systems#Ojibwe syllabics, syll.: ᐅᒋᐺ; plural: ''Ojibweg'' ᐅᒋᐺᒃ) are an Anishinaabe people whose homeland (''Ojibwewaki'' ᐅᒋᐺᐘᑭ) covers much of the Great Lakes region and the Great Plains, n ...
in which the obviative is marked on
nouns and
demonstratives and reflected in pronominal verb affixes:
That example shows that the proximate referent need not necessarily be the subject of a clause.
Potawatomi
Potawatomi (an Algonquian Language) is notable for having two degrees of obviation, "obviation" and "further obviation." "Further obviation" is rare, but when it occurs, a "further obviative" referent, deemed to be even less salient than the obviative referent, can be marked by an additional obviative suffix. The following is the sole example to appear in the literature on Potawatomi:
Charles Hockett
posited the following example, but he never checked it to see if it was grammatical:
Ingush
Obviation in
Ingush, a heavily
dependent-marking language, is an exception to the generalization that the obviative occurs in
head-marking languages. Obviation is not overtly marked in Ingush but is implied, as certain constructions are impossible unless one referent has salience over another.
For example, if a non-subject-referent has salience over the subject and precedes the other co-referent, reflexivisation (normally used only when there is a coreferent to the subject) is possible. That is shown in the example below whose non-subject-referent appears to have salience over the subject:
If the subject is salient ("proximate"), on the other hand, the subject's possessor does not antecede the third-person object, and the possession must be indirectly implicated as follows:
See also
*
Direct–inverse language
References
* {{Cite journal , last=Aissen , first=Judith , year=1997 , title=On the syntax of obviation , journal=Language , volume=73 , issue=4 , pages=705–750 , doi=10.1353/lan.1997.0042
External links
Obviation explained with a "spotlighting" analogy (starting on page 5, section 3)Obviation in Mi'gmaqHow exactly does an obviative proximate system work?
Grammatical categories