William Labov ( ;
born December 4, 1927) is an
American linguist, widely regarded as the founder of the discipline of variationist
sociolinguistics. He has been described as "an enormously original and influential figure who has created much of the methodology" of sociolinguistics. He is a
professor emeritus in the
linguistics department of the
University of Pennsylvania, and pursues research in sociolinguistics, language change, and
dialectology. He retired in 2015, but continues to publish research.
Biography
Born in
Rutherford, New Jersey, Labov majored in English and philosophy and studied chemistry at
Harvard (1948). He worked as an industrial chemist in his family’s business (1949–61) before turning to linguistics. For his MA thesis (1963) he completed a study of change in the dialect of
Martha's Vineyard, which was presented before the
Linguistic Society of America. Labov took his PhD (1964) at
Columbia University studying under
Uriel Weinreich. He was an assistant professor of linguistics at Columbia (1964–70) before becoming an associate professor at the
University of Pennsylvania in 1971, then a full professor, and in 1976 becoming director of the university's Linguistics Laboratory.
The five children of his first marriage to Teresa Gnasso Labov are Susannah Page, Sarah Labov, Simon Labov, Joanna Labov and Jessie Labov. He has been married to fellow sociolinguist
Gillian Sankoff since 1993 and they have two children: Rebecca Labov and sociologist
Alice Goffman.
Work
The methods he used to collect data for his study of the varieties of
English spoken in
New York City, published as ''The Social Stratification of English in New York City'' (1966), have been influential in social dialectology. In the late 1960s and early 1970s, his studies of the linguistic features of
African American Vernacular English (AAVE) were also influential: he argued that AAVE should not be stigmatized as substandard, but respected as a variety of English with its own grammatical rules. He has also pursued research in
referential indeterminacy, and is noted for his seminal studies of the way ordinary people structure narrative stories of their own lives. In addition, several of his classes are service-based, with students going to West Philadelphia to help tutor young children while simultaneously learning linguistics from different dialects such as AAVE.
More recently he has studied ongoing changes in the phonology of English as spoken in the United States, and the origins and patterns of
chain shifts of vowels (one sound replacing a second, replacing a third, in a complete chain). In the ''Atlas of North American English'' (2006), he and his co-authors find three major divergent chain shifts taking place today: a
Southern Shift (in
Appalachia and southern coastal regions), a
Northern Cities Vowel Shift affecting a region from
Madison, Wisconsin, east to
Utica, New York, and a
Canadian Shift affecting most of Canada, as well as some areas in the Western and Midwestern (Midland) United States, in addition to several minor chain shifts in smaller regions.
Among Labov's well-known students are
Charles Boberg,
Anne H. Charity Hudley,
Penelope Eckert,
Gregory Guy,
Robert A. Leonard,
Geoffrey Nunberg,
Shana Poplack, and
John R. Rickford. His methods were adopted in England by
Peter Trudgill for Norwich speech and
K. M. Petyt for West Yorkshire speech.
Labov's works include ''The Study of Nonstandard English'' (1969), ''Language in the Inner City: Studies in Black English Vernacular'' (1972), ''Sociolinguistic Patterns'' (1972), ''Principles of Linguistic Change'' (vol.I Internal Factors, 1994; vol.II Social Factors, 2001, vol.III Cognitive and Cultural factors, 2010), and, with
Sharon Ash and
Charles Boberg''The Atlas of North American English''(2006).
Labov was awarded the 2013
Benjamin Franklin Medal in Computer and Cognitive Science by the
Franklin Institute with the citation "
r establishing the cognitive basis of language variation and change through rigorous analysis of linguistic data, and for the study of non-standard dialects with significant social and cultural implications."
Language in use
In "Narrative Analysis: Oral Versions of Personal Experience", Labov, with
Joshua Waletzky, takes a sociolinguistic approach to examining how language works between people. This is significant because it contextualizes the study of structure and form, connecting purpose to method. His stated purpose is to "isolate the elements of narrative". This work focuses exclusively on oral narratives.
Labov describes narrative as having two functions: referential and evaluative, with its ''referential'' functions orienting and grounding a story in its contextual world by referencing events in sequential order as they originally occurred,
[Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1997). "Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience." p. 32.] and its ''evaluative'' functions describing the storyteller's purpose in telling the story.
[Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1997). "Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience." p. 41.] Formally analyzing data from orally generated texts obtained via observed group interaction and interview (600 interviews were taken from several studies whose participants included ethnically diverse groups of children and adults from various backgrounds), Labov divides narrative into five or six sections:
* ''Abstract'' – gives an overview of the story.
* ''Orientation'' – Labov describes this as "referential
ree clauses thatserve to orient the listener in respect to person, place, time, and behavioral situation".
He specifies that these are contextual clues that precede the main story.
* ''Complication'' – the main story, during which the narrative unfolds. A story may consist of multiple complication sections.
* ''Evaluation'' – author evinces self-awareness, giving explicit or implicit purpose to the retelling of the story. Thus evaluation gives some indication of the significance the author attributes to their story. But evaluation can be done subtly: for instance, "lexical intensifiers
re a type ofsemantically defined evaluation".
* ''Resolution'' – occurs sequentially following the evaluation. The resolution may give the story a sense of completion.
* ''Coda'' – returns listener to the present, drawing them back out of the world of the story into the world of the storytelling event. A coda is not essential to a narrative, and some narratives do not have one.
While not every narrative includes all these elements, the purpose of this subdivision is to show that narratives have inherent structural order. Labov argues that narrative units must retell events in the order they were experienced because narrative is ''temporally sequenced''. In other words, events do not occur at random but are connected to one another; thus "the original semantic interpretation" depends on their original order. To demonstrate this sequence, he breaks a story down into its basic parts. He defines ''narrative clause'' as the "basic unit of narrative" around which everything else is built. Clauses can be distinguished from one another by ''temporal junctures'', which indicate a shift in time and separate narrative clauses. Temporal junctures mark temporal sequencing because clauses cannot be rearranged without disrupting their meaning.
Labov and Waletzky's findings are important because they derived them from actual data rather than abstract theorization (a descriptive rather than a prescriptive approach). Labov, Waletzky, &c., set up interviews and documented speech patterns in storytelling, keeping with the ethnographic tradition of tape-recording oral text so it can be referenced exactly. This inductive method creates a new system through which to understand story text.
Golden Age Principle
One of Labov's most quoted contributions to theories of
language change is his ''Golden Age Principle'' (or ''Golden Age Theory''). It claims that any changes in the sounds or the grammar that have come to conscious awareness in a
speech community trigger a uniformly negative reaction.
Scholarly influence and criticism
Labov's seminal work has been referenced and critically examined by a number of scholars, mainly for its structural rigidity.
Kristin Langellier explains that "the purpose of Labovian analysis is to relate the formal properties of the narrative to their functions": clause-level analysis of how text affects transmission of message. This model has several flaws, which Langellier points out: it examines textual structure to the exclusion of context and audience, which often act to shape a text in real time; it's relevant to a specific demographic (may be difficult to extrapolate); and, by categorizing the text at a clausal level, it burdens analysis with theoretical distinctions that may not be illuminating in practice.
Anna De Fina remarks that
ithin Labov's model"the defining property of narrative is temporal sequence, since the order in which the events are presented in the narrative is expected to match the original events as they occurred", which differs from more contemporary notions of storytelling, in which a naturally time-conscious flow includes jumping forward and back in time as mandated by, for example, anxieties felt about futures and their interplay with subsequent decisions. De Fina and Langellier both note that, though wonderfully descriptive, Labov's model is nevertheless difficult to code, thus potentially limited in application/practice. De Fina also agrees with Langellier that Labov's model ignores the complex and often quite relevant subject of intertextuality in narrative. To an extent, Labov evinces awareness of these concerns, saying "it is clear that these conclusions are restricted to the speech communities that we have examined",
and "the overall structure of the narratives we've examined is not uniform". In "Rethinking Ventriloquism,"
Diane Goldstein uses Labovian notions of
tellability—internal coherence in narrative—to inform her concept of ''untellability''.
Honors
In 1968 Labov received th
David H. Russell Award for Distinguished Research in Teaching English
He was a
Guggenheim Fellow in 1970-71 and 1987-88.
Labov has received
honorary doctorates from, among others, the Faculty of Humanities at
Uppsala University (1985) and
University of Edinburgh (2005).
In 1996, he won th
Leonard Bloomfield Book Awardfrom the
Linguistic Society of America (LSA) for ''Principles of Linguistic Change, Vol. 1''.;
he won the Award again in 2008 as a coauthor of the ''Atlas of North American English''.
[
In 2013 Labov received a Franklin Institute Award in Computer and Cognitive Science for "establishing the cognitive basis of language variation and change through rigorous analysis of linguistic data, and for the study of non-standard dialects with significant social and cultural implications."
In 2015 he was awarded the Neil and Saras Smith Medal for Linguistics by the British Academy "for lifetime achievement in the scholarly study of linguistics" and "his significant contribution to linguistics and the language sciences".
In 2020, Labov will be awarded the American Academy of Arts and Sciences' Talcott Parsons Prize, recognizing "distinguished and original contributions to the social sciences".]
References
External links
William Labov's home page
''Journal of English Linguistics'' interview
NPR story "American Accent Undergoing Great Vowel Shift"
Sociolinguistics: an interview with William Labov
ReVEL, vol. 5, n. 9, 2007.
{{DEFAULTSORT:Labov, William
Category:1927 births
Category:Living people
Category:Linguists from the United States
Category:Sociolinguists
Category:Dialectologists
Category:Harvard College alumni
Category:Columbia University alumni
Category:University of Pennsylvania faculty
Category:Columbia University faculty
Category:Jewish American scientists
Category:Members of the United States National Academy of Sciences
Category:Fellows of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences
Category:Fellows of the American Association for the Advancement of Science
Category:People from Rutherford, New Jersey
Category:20th-century linguists
Category:21st-century linguists
Category:Fellows of the Cognitive Science Society
Category:Recipients of the Neil and Saras Smith Medal for Linguistics
Category:Linguistic Society of America presidents