Sluicing
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

In syntax, sluicing is a type of
ellipsis The ellipsis (, also known informally as dot dot dot) is a series of dots that indicates an intentional omission of a word, sentence, or whole section from a text without altering its original meaning. The plural is ellipses. The term origin ...
that occurs in both direct and indirect interrogative clauses. The ellipsis is introduced by a ''wh''-expression, whereby in most cases, everything except the ''wh''-expression is elided from the clause. Sluicing has been studied in detail in the early 21st century and it is therefore a relatively well-understood type of ellipsis. Sluicing occurs in many languages.See Merchant's (2001) extensive account of sluicing; it includes examples from numerous languages.


Basic examples

Sluicing is illustrated with the following examples. In each case, an embedded question is understood though only a question word or phrase is pronounced. (The intended interpretations of the question-denoting elliptical clause are given in parentheses; parts of these are anaphoric to the boldface material in the antecedent.) ::Phoebe ate something, but she doesn't know what. (=what she ate) ::Jon doesn't like the lentils, but he doesn't know why. (=why he doesn't like the lentils) ::Someone has eaten the soup. Unfortunately, I don't know who. (=who has eaten the soup) Sluicing in these examples occurs in indirect questions. It is also frequent in direct questions across speakers, e.g. ::Somebody is coming for dinner tonight. - Who? (=Who is coming for dinner tonight)? ::They put something in the mailbox. - What? (=What did they put in the mailbox)? The examples of sluicing above have the sluiced material following its antecedent. This material can also precede its antecedent, e.g. ::I don't know why, but the pictures have been moved. (=why the pictures have been moved) ::When and how is unclear, but somebody should say something. (=when and how somebody should say something) Jason Merchant (2001) states that these and other examples of sluicing can be organized into four categories of sluicing constructions. These types include sluices with adjunct wh-phrases, sluices with overt correlates, sluices with implicit arguments and contrast sluices. The first type refers to when the wh-phrase does not have an elided copy of the antecedent but is an adjunct. The following example from Ali Algryani (2019) shows this: : ::Zayd rāḥ, lakǝn ma-adri mita /wein. ::Zayd left.3MS but NEG-know.1S when/where ::‘Zayd left, but I don’t know when/where.’ Material was copied from this source, which is available under
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License
: The second type refers to a correlate in the antecedent clause that is indefinite. This is shown in the above example about someone eating the soup, with ‘someone’ being the indefinite correlate of ‘who’. The third type of sluicing construction refers to when the wh-word is not referring to a term in the antecedent but is referring to an object that corresponds to the preceding verb. The following example from Algryani (2019) shows this: : ::Fatema təqra, lakǝn ma-ʕaraf eiš. ::Fatema read.3FS but NEG-know.1S what ::‘Fatema is reading, but I don’t know what.’ : The final type of sluicing construction occurs when the elided material correspondent contrasts that of what is in the antecedent. The following example from Algryani (2019) shows this: : ::Zayd ʕand-ah walad, lakǝn ma-adri kam bent. ::Zayd has.3MS son but NEG-know.1S how many daughter ::‘Zayd has a son, but I don’t know how many daughters.’ :


Theoretical approaches to sluicing

There are two theoretical approaches that have been proposed for how sluicing occurs in languages. Ross (1969) is the first examination of sluicing; he argued that sluicing involves regular wh-fronting followed by deletion of the sister constituent of the wh-phrase. This analysis has been expanded in greater detail in Merchant (2001), the most comprehensive treatise on sluicing to date. A second kind of analysis is represented by Ginzburg and Sag (2000) and Culicover and Jackendoff (2005), both of which present nonstructural analyses of ellipsis, and do not posit unpronounced elliptical material. Yet another account of sluicing builds on the catena unit; the elided material is a catena.


Movement approach

The movement approach states that sluicing is a product of the syntactic derivation in which an embedded clause is built in the syntax and then the wh-phrase within the embedded clause moves outside of the constituent to the position of SpecCP (specifier to the complementizer phrase). These steps are then followed by the deletion (and therefore non-pronunciation) of the tense phrase node that contains the rest of the clause. Evidence for this approach is seen in the connectivity effects of case marking, binding and preposition stranding as outlined in Merchant (2001).


Case-marking in sluicing

Interrogative phrases in languages with morphological case-marking show the case appropriate to the understood verb as Ross, (1969) and Merchant, (2001), illustrated here with the German verb "schmeicheln" (to flatter), which governs the dative case on its object. :: Er hat jemandem geschmeichelt, aber ich weiß nicht, wem. ::he has someone.DAT flattered but I know not who.DAT ::"He flattered someone, but I don't know who." The sluiced wh-phrase must bear the same case that its counterpart in a non-elided structure would bear Merchant, (2001).


Preposition-stranding in sluicing

It has been concluded that languages that forbid preposition-stranding in question formation also forbid it in sluicing Merchant (2001), Stjepanovic, (2008), as in the following example from German: ::Er hat mit jemandem gesprochen, aber ich weiß nicht, *(mit) wem. ::he has with someone spoken but I know not with who ::"He spoke with someone, but I don't know (with) who." Examples of languages where p-stranding does not occur are Greek, German, and Russian. Much research has been done to determine if sluicing can allow for preposition-stranding in a non-preposition-stranding language. Stjepanović, (2008) conducted research on whether this is possible in the non-preposition-stranding language, Serbo-Croatian. She concluded that there is not enough evidence to contradict the initial claim made by Ross. However, she did find that a preposition may be lost or removed from a sentence under sluicing in Serbo-Croatian. More research is to be conducted to confirm the official cause of this preposition-loss. An example of the preposition-loss shown by Stjepanović, (2008) is displayed below. ::Petar je sakrio igradku ispod jedne stolice i pored jednog zida, ali ne znam (ispod) koje stolice i (pored) kojeg zida. ::Gloss Petar is hidden toy under one chair.GEN and beside one wall. gen but not I.know under which chair.GEN and beside which wall.GEN 4 ::Translation Petar hid the toy under a chair and beside a wall, but I don't know which chair and which wall.'


Binding

Merchant (2003) demonstrates that binding supports the movement approach using the following sentence: ::Every linguist1  criticized some of his1 work, but I’m not sure how much of his1 work very linguist1 criticized ''t''/s> In order for the second “his work” to refer to “every linguist” in the above example, it must be c-commanded by its antecedent within its local domain. Here, “his work” could not be coreferential with the subject: “every linguist” at the beginning of the sentence because it is outside of its local domain. This provides evidence that “his work” originally started off in the elided constituent where it could be c-commanded and in the local domain of that “every linguist” before it moved out of the clause.


Non-movement approach

There are also several theoretical approaches to sluicing that do not involve the movement of the wh-phrase out of the embedded clause. These approaches include PF deletion and LF copying. PF deletion as proposed in Lasnik (2007) states that the TP within the embedded clause is null and has syntactic structure within it that is elided following a wh-movement operation. The other approach, LF copying, is a process proposed by Lobeck (1995) in which the original structure of a sluicing phrase is one in which the wh-word originates in the SpecCP position of the embedded clause and a null phrase marker (marked ''e'') occupies the position of the tense phrase of the embedded clause. This is the extent of the syntactic derivation. After this structure is derived, it is sent off for semantic interpretation, to logical form, in which the implied material in the tense phrase is then present for our full understanding of the sentence. The evidence for this approach is that it is able to account for islands in sluicing as is discussed below.


Islands in sluicing

Sluicing has garnered considerable attention because it appears, as Ross (1969) first discussed, to allow wh-fronting to violate the
island An island (or isle) is an isolated piece of habitat that is surrounded by a dramatically different habitat, such as water. Very small islands such as emergent land features on atolls can be called islets, skerries, cays or keys. An island ...
conditions he discovered: :: They want to hire someone who speaks a Balkan language, but I don't remember which one. (=*which one they want to hire someone who speaks) Sluicing allows a sentence that contains an island to retain its meaning and remain grammatical. As mentioned by Abels, (2018), there is an ongoing debate on whether this can happen in all situations or if it is island dependent. ::A biography of one of the Marx brothers will be published later this year, guess which (of the Marx brothers) biography of which of the Marx brothers/s> will be published later this year. ::A biography of one of the Marx brothers will be published later this year, guess which. The first example is ungrammatical because the island prevents us from moving anything out of the subject constituent (shown in square brackets). The second example is saved through sluicing as the island is sluiced and the meaning can be inferred from the context of the sentence, therefore maintaining the meaning and remaining grammatical.


Multiple sluicing

In some languages, sluicing can leave behind more than one ''wh''-phrase (multiple remnant sluicing): ::Someone wants to eat something. ?I wish I knew who what. (=who wants to eat what) ::?Something is causing someone big problems, although it's not clear what who. (=what is causing who big problems) Sentences like these are considered acceptable in languages like German, Japanese, Chinese, Turkish, Russian, and others, although in English, their acceptability seems marginal (but see Bolinger 1978, Merchant 2001, and Richards 2010 for examples). Lasnik 2014 discusses the fact that the wh-phrase remnants in multiple sluicing must be clausemates: :: *Someone told me that something broke, but I don't remember who what. (≠who told me that what broke)


Issues with different approaches to sluicing

Only the catena-based approach handles multiple sluicing without further elaboration. The structural movement analysis must rely on some other type of movement to evacuate the noninitial wh-phrase from the ellipsis site; proposals for this additional movement include extraposition or shifting and need to be able to account for islands in sluicing. The nonstructural analysis must add phrase-structure rules to allow an interrogative clause to consist of multiple wh-phrases and be able to account for connectivity effects. The catena-based approach, however, does not account for the locality facts; since catenae can span multiple clauses, the fact that multiply-sluiced wh-phrases must be clausemates is a mystery.


Sluicing in other languages


Omani Arabic

Sluicing has also been analyzed in Omani Arabi as is shown in Algryani (2019). All four of the above stated sluicing constructions outlined by Merchant (2001) are accounted for in Omani Arabic. Algryani (2019) displays the different constructions in the following examples: Sluices with Adjunct Wh-Phrases ::Zayd rāḥ, lakǝn ma-adri mita /wein. ::Zayd left.3MS but NEG-know.1S when/where ::‘Zayd left, but I don’t know when/where.’ Sluices with Overt Correlates ::Zaid qabǝl ḥad, lakǝn ma-aʕraf mi:n. ::Zaid met.3MS someone but NEG-know.1S. who ::‘Zaid met someone, but I don’t know who.’ Sluices with Implicit Arguments ::Fatema təqra, lakǝn ma-ʕaraf eiš. ::Fatema read.3FS but NEG-know.1S what ::‘Fatema is reading, but I don’t know what.’ Contrast Sluices ::Zayd ʕand-ah walad, lakǝn ma-adri kam bent. ::Zayd has.3MS son but NEG-know.1S how many daughter ::‘Zayd has a son, but I don’t know how many daughters.’


Danish

The following example from Merchant, (2003) displays sluicing in Danish: ::Peter har snakket med en eller anden, men jeg ved ikke hvem. ::Peter has talked with one or another but I know not who. ::Peter has talked with someone, but I don't know who.


German

The following example from Abels, (2018) displays sluicing in German: ::Hans will jemandem helfen, aber ich weiß nicht wem. ::Hans wants someone help but I know not whom. ::Hans wants to help someone, but I don't know whom.


Japanese

The following example from Merchant (2003) displays sluicing in Japanese: ::Abby-ga dareka-o mi-ta ga, watashi-wa dare ka wakaranai. ::Abby-NOM someone-ACC see-PAST but I-TOP who Q know.not ::‘Abby saw someone, but I don’t know who.’


Korean

The following example from
Kim Kim or KIM may refer to: Names * Kim (given name) * Kim (surname) ** Kim (Korean surname) *** Kim family (disambiguation), several dynasties **** Kim family (North Korea), the rulers of North Korea since Kim Il-sung in 1948 ** Kim, Vietnamese f ...
& Sells (2013) displays sluicing in Korean: ::Mimi-ka khu-n cha-lul sa-ss-nuntey, elmana khu-nci molukeyssta ::Mimi-NOM big-MOD car-ACC buy-PAST-but how big-QCOMP not.know ::‘Mimi bought a big car, but I don’t know how big.’


See also

*
Ellipsis (linguistics) In linguistics, ellipsis (from el, ἔλλειψις, ''élleipsis'' 'omission') or an elliptical construction is the omission from a clause of one or more words that are nevertheless understood in the context of the remaining elements. There a ...
*
Verb phrase ellipsis In linguistics, verb phrase ellipsis (VP-ellipsis or VPE) is a type of elliptical construction and a type of anaphora in which a verb phrase has been left out (elided) provided that its antecedent can be found within the same linguistic context. ...


Notes

{{Reflist


References

*Abels, K. 2018. Movement and Islands. The Oxford Handbook of Ellipsis, 16, 1-43. *Bolinger, Dwight. 1978. Asking more than one thing at a time. In Henry Hiz (ed.), Questions, 107–150. Reidel: Dordrecht. *Chiu, Liching. 2007. A Focus Movement Account on Multiple Sluicing in Mandarin Chinese Nanzan Linguistics. Special Issue Vol. 1. P.23-31. *Chung, Sandra, William Ladusaw, and James McCloskey. 1995. Sluicing and Logical Form. ''Natural Language Semantics'' 3, 239–282. *Culicover, Peter and Ray Jackendoff. 2005. Simpler Syntax. Oxford University Press: Oxford. *Ginzburg, Jonathan and Ivan Sag. 2000. Interrogative Investigations. CSLI Publications: Stanford, Calif. *Kim, J. B., & Sells, P. (2013). Sluicing in Korean. Talk Presented at Structure and Evidence in Linguistics. Sanford University, CA. *Lasnik, Howard. 2007. On Ellipsis: The PF approach to missing constituents. In A. Conroy, C. Jing, C. Nakao & E. Takahashi (Eds.), Working Papers in Linguistics 15 (pp. 143–153). University of Maryland, College Park. *Lasnik, Howard. 2014. Multiple sluicing in English? ''Syntax'' 17, 1, 1-20. *Lobeck, Anne. 1995. Ellipsis: Functional heads, licensing, and identification. Oxford University Press: Oxford. *Merchant, Jason. 2001. The syntax of silence: Sluicing, identity, and the theory of ellipsis. Oxford University Press: Oxford. *Merchant, J. (2003). SynCom Case 98 Sluicing *Osborne, Timothy, Michael Putnam, and Thomas Groß 2013. Catenae: Introducing a novel unit of syntactic analysis. ''Syntax'' 15, 4, 354–396. *Osborne, Timothy (to appear). Dependency grammar. In ''The Oxford Handbook of Ellipsis''. Oxford University Press. *Richards, Norvin. 2010. Uttering trees. MIT Press: Cambridge, Mass. *Ross, John R. 1969. Guess who? in ''CLS 5: Papers from the fifth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society'', eds. Robert Binnick, Alice Davison, Georgia Green, and Jerry Morgan, 252–286. Chicago, Illinois: Chicago Linguistic Society. *Stjepanović, S. 2008. P-stranding under Sluicing in a Non-P-Stranding Language? Linguistic Inquiry, 39(1), 179–190. Syntactic entities Syntactic transformation