Force dynamics
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Force dynamics is a
semantic Semantics (from grc, σημαντικός ''sēmantikós'', "significant") is the study of reference, meaning, or truth. The term can be used to refer to subfields of several distinct disciplines, including philosophy, linguistics and comput ...
category that describes the way in which entities interact with reference to force. Force Dynamics gained a good deal of attention in
cognitive linguistics Cognitive linguistics is an interdisciplinary branch of linguistics, combining knowledge and research from cognitive science, cognitive psychology, neuropsychology and linguistics. Models and theoretical accounts of cognitive linguistics are con ...
due to its claims of psychological plausibility and the elegance with which it generalizes ideas not usually considered in the same context. The semantic category of force dynamics pervades
language Language is a structured system of communication. The structure of a language is its grammar and the free components are its vocabulary. Languages are the primary means by which humans communicate, and may be conveyed through a variety of ...
on several levels. Not only does it apply to expressions in the physical domain like ''leaning on'' or ''dragging'', but it also plays an important role in expressions involving psychological forces (e.g. ''wanting'' or ''being urged''). Furthermore, the concept of force dynamics can be extended to
discourse Discourse is a generalization of the notion of a conversation to any form of communication. Discourse is a major topic in social theory, with work spanning fields such as sociology, anthropology, continental philosophy, and discourse analysis. ...
. For example, the situation in which speakers A and B argue, after which speaker A gives in to speaker B, exhibits a force dynamic pattern.


Context

Introduced by cognitive linguist
Leonard Talmy Leonard Talmy is an emeritus professor of linguistics Linguistics is the scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of ...
in 1981, force dynamics started out as a generalization of the traditional notion of the
causative In linguistics, a causative ( abbreviated ) is a valency-increasing operationPayne, Thomas E. (1997). Describing morphosyntax: A guide for field linguists'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 173–186. that indicates that a subject either ...
, dividing ''causation'' into finer primitives and considering the notions of ''letting'', ''hindering'', and ''helping''. Talmy further developed the field in his 1985, 1988 and 2000 works. Talmy places force dynamics within the broader context of
cognitive semantics Cognitive semantics is part of the cognitive linguistics movement. Semantics is the study of linguistic meaning. Cognitive semantics holds that language is part of a more general human cognitive ability, and can therefore only describe the world a ...
. In his view, a general idea underlying this discipline is the existence of a fundamental distinction in language between closed-class (
grammatical In linguistics, grammaticality is determined by the conformity to language usage as derived by the grammar of a particular variety (linguistics), speech variety. The notion of grammaticality rose alongside the theory of generative grammar, the go ...
) and open-class ( lexical) categories. This distinction is motivated by the fact that language uses certain categories of notions to structure and organize meaning, while other categories are excluded from this function. For example, Talmy remarks that many languages mark the
number A number is a mathematical object used to count, measure, and label. The original examples are the natural numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, and so forth. Numbers can be represented in language with number words. More universally, individual number ...
of
nouns A noun () is a word that generally functions as the name of a specific object or set of objects, such as living creatures, places, actions, qualities, states of existence, or ideas.Example nouns for: * Living creatures (including people, alive, ...
in a systematic way, but that nouns are not marked in the same way for ''color''. Force Dynamics is considered to be one of the closed-class notional categories, together with such generally recognized categories as number, aspect, mood, and
evidentiality In linguistics, evidentiality is, broadly, the indication of the nature of evidence for a given statement; that is, whether evidence exists for the statement and if so, what kind. An evidential (also verificational or validational) is the particul ...
. Aspects of force dynamics have been incorporated into the theoretical frameworks of Mark Johnson (1987),
Steven Pinker Steven Arthur Pinker (born September 18, 1954) is a Canadian-American cognitive psychologist, psycholinguist, popular science author, and public intellectual. He is an advocate of evolutionary psychology and the computational theory of mind. ...
(1997) and
Ray Jackendoff Ray Jackendoff (born January 23, 1945) is an American linguist. He is professor of philosophy, Seth Merrin Chair in the Humanities and, with Daniel Dennett, co-director of the Center for Cognitive Studies at Tufts University. He has always s ...
(1990) (see Deane 1996 for a critical review of Jackendoff’s version of Force Dynamics). Force dynamics plays an important role in several recent accounts of modal verbs in various languages (including Brandt 1992, Achard 1996, Boye 2001, and Vandenberghe 2002). Other applications of force dynamics include use in
discourse analysis Discourse analysis (DA), or discourse studies, is an approach to the analysis of written, vocal, or sign language use, or any significant semiotic event. The objects of discourse Analysis (discourse, writing, conversation, communicative event) ...
(Talmy 1988, 2000),
lexical semantics Lexical semantics (also known as lexicosemantics), as a subfield of linguistic semantics, is the study of word meanings.Pustejovsky, J. (2005) Lexical Semantics: Overview' in Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, second edition, Volumes 1-14Ta ...
(Deane 1992, Da Silva 2003) and morphosyntactical analysis (Chun & Zubin 1990, Langacker 1999:352-4).


Theoretical outline


Basic concepts

Expressions can exhibit a force dynamic pattern or can be force-dynamically neutral. A sentence like ''The door is closed'' is force-dynamically neutral, because there are no forces opposing each other. The sentence ''The door cannot open'', on the other hand, exhibits a force dynamic pattern: apparently the door has some tendency toward opening, but there is some other force preventing it from being opened (e.g., it may be jammed). A basic feature of a force-dynamic expression is the presence of two force-exerting elements. Languages make a distinction between these two forces based on their roles. The force entity that is in focus is called the ''agonist'' and the force entity opposing it is the ''Antagonist'' (see ''a, figure 1''). In the example, the ''door'' is the agonist and the force preventing the door from being opened is the Antagonist. Force entities have an ''intrinsic force tendency'', either toward action or toward rest. For the agonist, this tendency is marked with an arrowhead (action) or with a large dot (rest) (see ''b, figure 1''). Since the antagonist by definition has an opposing tendency, it need not be marked. In the example, the door has a tendency toward action. A third relevant factor is the balance between the two forces. The forces are out of balance by definition; if the two forces are equally strong, the situation is not interesting from a force-dynamic point of view. One force is therefore stronger or weaker than the other. A stronger force is marked with a ''plus'' sign, a weaker force with a ''minus'' sign (''c, figure 1''). In the example, the Antagonist is stronger, since it actually holds back the door. The outcome of the Force-Dynamic scenario depends on both the intrinsic tendency and the balance between the forces. The result is represented by a line beneath Agonist and Antagonist. The line has an arrowhead if the outcome is action and a large dot if the outcome is rest (''d, figure 1''). In the example, the door stays closed; the Antagonist succeeds in preventing it from being opened. The sentence 'The door cannot open' can be Force-Dynamically represented by the diagram at the top of this page. Using these basic concepts, several generalizations can be made. The force dynamic situations in which the Agonist is stronger are expressed in sentences like ‘X happened ''despite'' Y’, while situations in which the Antagonist is stronger are expressed in the form of ‘X happened ''because of'' Y’. In the latter, a form of causation that Talmy termed ''extended causation'' is captured.


More complexity

More possibilities arise when another variable is introduced: ''change over time''. This variable is exemplified by such expressions as ''A gust of wind made the pages of my book turn''. In force dynamic terms, the situation can be described as the entering of an antagonist (the wind) that is stronger in force than the agonist (the pages) and changes the force tendency of the pages from a state of rest to a state of action (turning). In force dynamic diagrams, this motion (‘change over time’) of the Antagonist is represented by an arrow. The diagrams in ''Figure 2'' to the right combine a shifting antagonist with agonists of varying force tendencies. The following sentences are examples for these patterns: :a. ''A gust of wind made the pages of my book turn.'' :b. ''The appearance of the headmaster made the pupils calm down.'' :c. ''The breaking of the dam let the water flow from the storage lake.'' :d. ''The abating of the wind let the sailboat slow down.'' In this series of scenarios, various kinds of causation are described. Furthermore, a basic relationship between the concepts of ‘causing something to happen’ and ‘letting something happen’ emerges, definable in terms of the balance between the force entities and the resultants of the interaction. Force entities do not have to be physical entities. Force dynamics is directly applicable to terms involving psychological forces like ''to persuade'' and ''to urge''. The force dynamic aspect of the sentence ''Herbie did not succeed in persuading Diana to sing another song'' can be graphically represented as easily as the earlier example sentence ''The door cannot open'' (and, incidentally, by the same diagram). In addition, force entities do not have to be physically separate. A case in point is ''reflexive'' force dynamic constructions of the type ''Chet was dragging himself instead of walking''. It is perfectly possible to represent this in a Force Dynamic diagram (representing Chet’s will as the Agonist keeping the body — the Antagonist — in motion). Thus, even though Chet is one person, his will and his body are conceptualized separately.


Psychological basis

The key elements of force dynamics are very basic to
human cognition Cognition refers to "the mental action or process of acquiring knowledge and understanding through thought, experience, and the senses". It encompasses all aspects of Intellect, intellectual functions and processes such as: perception, attentio ...
. Deane (1996:56) commented that “ om a cognitive perspective, Talmy’s theory is a striking example of a psychologically plausible theory of causation. Its key elements are such concepts as the (amount of) force exerted by an entity, the balance between two such forces, and the force vector which results from their interaction. Such concepts have an obvious base in ordinary motor activities: the brain must be able to calculate the force vector produced by muscular exertion, and calculate the probable outcome when that force is exerted against an object in the outside world.” In cognitive linguistic terms, force dynamic expressions reflect a '' conceptual archetype'' because of their conceptual basality (Langacker 1999:24). In this view, expressions involving psychological forces reflect an extension of the category of force dynamics from the physical domain to the psychological domain.


Limitations and criticism

From the perspective of
lexical semantics Lexical semantics (also known as lexicosemantics), as a subfield of linguistic semantics, is the study of word meanings.Pustejovsky, J. (2005) Lexical Semantics: Overview' in Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, second edition, Volumes 1-14Ta ...
, some people have argued that force dynamics fails to be explanatory. For example, Goddard (1998:262–266) raised the objection that "a visual representation cannot — in and of itself — convey a meaning. (…) From a semiotic point of view, a diagram never stands alone; it always depends on a system of verbal captions, whether these are explicit or implied." He goes on to attack the verbal definition of causation Talmy provides, claiming that it is circular and obscure. Furthermore, Goddard objects to the use of the "semantically obscure concept of force". However, Goddard's objections lose some of their strength in light of the fact that Force Dynamics does not present itself as a complete semantic description of the constructions involving Force Dynamic concepts. Another objection regarding force dynamics is the question, raised by Goddard (1998:81), of how different representational devices are supposed to interact with one another. As the field of
cognitive linguistics Cognitive linguistics is an interdisciplinary branch of linguistics, combining knowledge and research from cognitive science, cognitive psychology, neuropsychology and linguistics. Models and theoretical accounts of cognitive linguistics are con ...
is still in a state of theoretical flux, no systematic account addresses this issue yet. However, it is an objection many cognitive linguists are aware of. Some cognitive linguists have replied to such objections by pointing out that the goal of Cognitive Linguistics is not to construct a
formal system A formal system is an abstract structure used for inferring theorems from axioms according to a set of rules. These rules, which are used for carrying out the inference of theorems from axioms, are the logical calculus of the formal system. A fo ...
in which theorems are proved, but rather to better understand the cognitive basis of language (
cf. The abbreviation ''cf.'' (short for the la, confer/conferatur, both meaning "compare") is used in writing to refer the reader to other material to make a comparison with the topic being discussed. Style guides recommend that ''cf.'' be used onl ...
Newman 1996:xii). Jackendoff (1990, 1996:120–3), in the process of incorporating aspects of force dynamics into his theory of conceptual semantics, has proposed a reconfiguration of some of its basic notions{{vague, date=September 2018. In Jackendoff’s view, this reconfiguration "conforms better to the syntax of force-dynamic verbs" (1996:121).


References


Primary sources

* Talmy, Leonard (2000) ‘Force Dynamics in Language and Cognition’ Chapter 7 of Talmy, ''Toward a cognitive semantics ''vol I: '' Concept structuring systems''. Cambridge: MIT Press. 'This chapter is a modestly rewritten version of'':* Talmy, Leonard (1988a) ‘Force Dynamics in language and cognition’ In ''Cognitive Science'', 12, 1, 49–100. 'This article is a moderately rewritten version of'':* Talmy, Leonard (1985a) ‘Force Dynamics in language and thought’ In ''Papers from the Regional Meetings, Chicago Linguistic Society'', 21, 293–337.


Secondary sources

* Achard, Michel (1996) ‘French modals and speaker control’ In Goldberg, Adele (ed.), ''Conceptual Structure, Discourse and Language''. Stanford, CA.: CSL&I. * Boye, Kasper (2001) ‘The Force-Dynamic core meaning of Danish modal verbs’ In ''Acta Linguistica Hafniensia'', 33, 19–66. * Brandt, Per Aage (1989) 'Agonistique et analyse dynamique catastrophiste du modal et de l’aspectuel: quelques remarques sur la linguistique cognitive de L. Talmy’ In ''Semiotica'', 77, 1–3, 151–162. * Brandt, Per Aage (1992) ''La charpente modale du sens: Pour une simio-linguistique morphogenitique et dynamique''. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. * Chun, Soon Ae & David A Zubin (1990) ‘Experiential vs. Agentive Constructions in Korean Narrative’. In ''Proceedings of the Berkeley linguistics Society'' 16, 81–93. * Deane, Paul D (1992) 'Polysemy as the consequence of internal conceptual complexity: the case of over’ In ''Proceedings of the Eastern States Conference on Linguistics (ESCOL) '', 9, 32–43. * Deane, Paul D (1996) ‘On Jackendoff’s conceptual semantics’ In ''Cognitive Linguistics'', 7, 1, 35–91. * Goddard, Cliff (1998) ‘‘Semantic Analysis: A Practical Introduction‘‘ New York: Oxford University Press. (esp p 262-266) * Jackendoff, Ray (1990) ''Semantic Structures''. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. * Jackendoff, Ray (1996) 'Conceptual semantics and cognitive linguistics’. In ''Cognitive Linguistics'', 7, 1, 93–129. * Johnson, Mark (1987). ''The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason'', University of Chicago. * Langacker, Ronald W. (1999) ''Grammar and Conceptualization.'' Cognitive Linguistics Research vol. 14. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. * Pinker, Steven. 1997. ''How the mind works''. New York: Norton. * Silva, Augusto Soares da (2003) ‘Image schemas and category coherence: the Case of the Portuguese Verb deixar’. In ''Cognitive Approaches to Lexical Semantics'', Cuyckens & Dirve & Taylor (eds.), 281–322. * Sweetser, Eve (1982) ‘A proposal for uniting deontic and epistemic modals. In ''Proceedings of the Eighth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society''. Berkeley, California: Berkeley Linguistics Society. * Sweetser, Eve (1984) ‘Semantic structure and semantic change: A cognitive linguistic study of modality, perception, speech acts, and logical relations. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley. * Talmy, Leonard (1976a) ‘Semantic causative types’ In Shibatani (ed.), ''Syntax and semantics '' (vol 6) : '' The grammar of causative constructions''. New York: Academic Press. * Talmy, Leonard (1981) ‘Force Dynamics’. Paper presented at conference on Language and Mental Imagery. May 1981, University of California, Berkeley. * Talmy, Leonard (1985b) ‘Force Dynamics as a generalization over causative’ In ''Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics'', 67–85. * Vandenberghe, Wim (2002) ‘Instigative Setting-Constructions: Force Dynamic Research on ‘New’ Types of Agency’ In ''Leuvense Bijdragen'', 90, 4, 365–390.


External links


Presentation of Force Dynamics
on the CogSci index.

— read-only online version of Talmy (2000) ''Toward a Cognitive Semantics''.
Force Dynamics in Language and Cognition
— direct link to the chapter on Force Dynamics on the above webpage (PDF). Semantics