Syllable body coda.png
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

A syllable is a unit of organization for a sequence of Phone (phonetics), speech sounds typically made up of a syllable nucleus (most often a vowel) with optional initial and final margins (typically, consonants). Syllables are often considered the Phonology, phonological "building blocks" of words. They can influence the Isochrony, rhythm of a language, its prosody (linguistics), prosody, its Metre (poetry), poetic metre and its stress (linguistics), stress patterns. Speech can usually be divided up into a whole number of syllables: for example, the word ''ignite'' is made of two syllables: ''ig'' and ''nite''. Syllabic writing began several hundred years before the Middle Bronze Age alphabets, first letters. The earliest recorded syllables are on tablets written around 2800 BC in the Sumerian city of Ur. This shift from pictograms to syllables has been called "the most important advance in the history of writing". A word that consists of a single syllable (like English language, English ''dog'') is called a monosyllable (and is said to be ''monosyllabic''). Similar terms include disyllable (and ''disyllabic''; also ''bisyllable'' and ''bisyllabic'') for a word of two syllables; trisyllable (and ''trisyllabic'') for a word of three syllables; and polysyllable (and ''polysyllabic''), which may refer either to a word of more than three syllables or to any word of more than one syllable.


Etymology

''Syllable'' is an Anglo-Norman language, Anglo-Norman variation of Old French ''sillabe'', from Latin ''syllaba'', from Koine Greek ''syllabḗ'' (). means "the taken together", referring to letters that are taken together to make a single sound. is a verbal noun from the verb ''syllambánō'', a compound of the preposition ''sýn'' "with" and the verb ''lambánō'' "take". The noun uses the Proto-Indo-European root, root , which appears in the Aorist (Ancient Greek), aorist tense; the Ancient Greek verbs#Present tense, present tense stem is formed by adding a nasal infix ' before the ''b'' and a suffix ''-an'' at the end.


Transcription

In the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), the period marks syllable breaks, as in the word "astronomical" . In practice, however, IPA transcription is typically divided into words by spaces, and often these spaces are also understood to be syllable breaks. In addition, the stress mark is placed immediately before a stressed syllable, and when the stressed syllable is in the middle of a word, in practice, the stress mark also marks a syllable break, for example in the word "understood" (though the syllable boundary may still be explicitly marked with a full stop, e.g. ). When a word space comes in the middle of a syllable (that is, when a syllable spans words), a tie bar can be used for liaison (French), liaison, as in the French combination ''les amis'' . The liaison tie is also used to join lexical words into phonological words, for example ''hot dog'' . A Greek sigma, , is used as a wild card for 'syllable', and a dollar/peso sign, , marks a syllable boundary where the usual period might be misunderstood. For example, is a pair of syllables, and is a syllable-final vowel.


Components


Typical model

In the typical theory of syllable structure, the general structure of a syllable (σ) consists of three segments. These segments are grouped into two components: ; #Onset, Onset (ω): a consonant or consonant cluster, obligatory in some languages, optional or even restricted in others ; #Rime, Rime (ρ): right branch, contrasts with onset, splits into nucleus and coda :; #Nucleus, Nucleus (ν): a vowel or syllabic consonant, obligatory in most languages :; #Coda, Coda (κ): a consonant or consonant cluster, optional in some languages, highly restricted or prohibited in others The syllable is usually considered right-branching, i.e. nucleus and coda are grouped together as a "rime" and are only distinguished at the second level. The ''nucleus'' is usually the vowel in the middle of a syllable. The ''onset'' is the sound or sounds occurring before the nucleus, and the ''coda'' (literally 'tail') is the sound or sounds that follow the nucleus. They are sometimes collectively known as the ''shell''. The term ''rime'' covers the nucleus plus coda. In the one-syllable English word ''cat'', the nucleus is ''a'' (the sound that can be shouted or sung on its own), the onset ''c'', the coda ''t'', and the rime ''at''. This syllable can be abstracted as a ''consonant-vowel-consonant'' syllable, abbreviated ''CVC''. Languages vary greatly in the restrictions on the sounds making up the onset, nucleus and coda of a syllable, according to what is termed a language's phonotactics. Although every syllable has supra-segmental features, these are usually ignored if not semantically relevant, e.g. in tonal languages. ; Tone (linguistics), Tone (τ): may be carried by the syllable as a whole or by the rime


Chinese model

In Sinitic Languages, Chinese syllable structure, the onset is replaced with an initial, and a semivowel or liquid forms another segment, called the medial. These four segments are grouped into two slightly different components: ; #initial, Initial (ι): optional onset, excluding sonorants ; #final, Final (φ): medial, nucleus, and final consonant :; #medial, Medial (μ): optional semivowel or liquid :; #Nucleus, Nucleus (ν): a vowel or syllabic consonant :; #Coda, Coda (κ): optional final consonant In many languages of the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area, such as Chinese language, Chinese, the syllable structure is expanded to include an additional, optional segment known as a medial, which is located between the onset (often termed the ''initial'' in this context) and the rime. The medial is normally a semivowel, but reconstructions of Old Chinese generally include liquid consonant, liquid medials ( in modern reconstructions, in older versions), and many reconstructions of Middle Chinese include a medial contrast between and , where the functions phonologically as a glide rather than as part of the nucleus. In addition, many reconstructions of both Old and Middle Chinese include complex medials such as , , and . The medial groups phonologically with the rime rather than the onset, and the combination of medial and rime is collectively known as the final. Some linguists, especially when discussing the modern Chinese varieties, use the terms "final" and "rime/rhyme" interchangeably. In historical Chinese phonology, however, the distinction between "final" (including the medial) and "rime" (not including the medial) is important in understanding the rime dictionary, rime dictionaries and rime tables that form the primary sources for Middle Chinese, and as a result most authors distinguish the two according to the above definition.


Grouping of components

In some theories of phonology, syllable structures are displayed as Parse tree, tree diagrams (similar to the trees found in some types of syntax). Not all phonologists agree that syllables have internal structure; in fact, some phonologists doubt the existence of the syllable as a theoretical entity. There are many arguments for a hierarchical relationship, rather than a linear one, between the syllable constituents. One hierarchical model groups the syllable nucleus and coda into an intermediate level, the ''rime''. The hierarchical model accounts for the role that the ''nucleus''+''coda'' constituent plays in verse (i.e., rhyming words such as ''cat'' and ''bat'' are formed by matching both the nucleus and coda, or the entire rime), and for the Syllable weight, distinction between heavy and light syllables, which plays a role in phonological processes such as, for example, sound change in Old English and .


Body

In some traditional descriptions of certain languages such as Cree language, Cree and Ojibwe language, Ojibwe, the syllable is considered left-branching, i.e. onset and nucleus group below a higher-level unit, called a "body" or "core". This contrasts with the coda.


Rime

The rime or rhyme of a syllable consists of a #Nucleus, nucleus and an optional #Coda, coda. It is the part of the syllable used in most Rhyme, poetic rhymes, and the part that is lengthened or stressed when a person elongates or stresses a word in speech. The rime is usually the portion of a syllable from the first vowel to the end. For example, is the rime of all of the words ''at'', ''sat'', and ''flat''. However, the nucleus does not necessarily need to be a vowel in some languages. For instance, the rime of the second syllables of the words ''bottle'' and ''fiddle'' is just , a liquid consonant. Just as the rime branches into the nucleus and coda, the nucleus and coda may each branch into multiple phonemes. The limit for the number of phonemes which may be contained in each varies by language. For example, Japanese language, Japanese and most Sino-Tibetan languages do not have consonant clusters at the beginning or end of syllables, whereas many Eastern European languages can have more than two consonants at the beginning or end of the syllable. In English, the onset, nucleus, and coda may all have two phonemes, as in the word ''flouts'': [fl] in the onset, the diphthong [aʊ] in the nucleus, and [ts] in the coda. ''Rime'' and ''rhyme'' are variants of the same word, but the rarer form ''rime'' is sometimes used to mean specifically ''syllable rime'' to differentiate it from the concept of poetic rhyme. This distinction is not made by some linguists and does not appear in most dictionaries.


Weight

A heavy syllable is generally one with a ''branching rime'', i.e. it is either a ''closed syllable'' that ends in a consonant, or a syllable with a ''branching nucleus'', i.e. a long vowel or diphthong. The name is a metaphor, based on the nucleus or coda having lines that branch in a tree diagram. In some languages, heavy syllables include both VV (branching nucleus) and VC (branching rime) syllables, contrasted with V, which is a light syllable. In other languages, only VV syllables are considered heavy, while both VC and V syllables are light. Some languages distinguish a third type of superheavy syllable, which consists of VVC syllables (with both a branching nucleus and rime) or VCC syllables (with a coda consisting of two or more consonants) or both. In mora (linguistics), moraic theory, heavy syllables are said to have two moras, while light syllables are said to have one and superheavy syllables are said to have three. Japanese phonology is generally described this way. Many languages forbid superheavy syllables, while a significant number forbid any heavy syllable. Some languages strive for constant syllable weight; for example, in stressed, non-final syllables in Italian language, Italian, short vowels co-occur with closed syllables while long vowels co-occur with open syllables, so that all such syllables are heavy (not light or superheavy). The difference between heavy and light frequently determines which syllables receive stress (linguistics), stress – this is the case in Latin and Arabic language, Arabic, for example. The system of meter (poetry), poetic meter in many classical languages, such as Classical Greek, Classical Latin, Tamil language, Old Tamil and Sanskrit language, Sanskrit, is based on syllable weight rather than stress (so-called ''quantitative rhythm'' or ''quantitative meter'').


Syllabification

Syllabification is the separation of a word into syllables, whether spoken or written. In most languages, the actually spoken syllables are the basis of syllabification in writing too. Due to the very weak correspondence between sounds and letters in the spelling of modern English, for example, written syllabification in English has to be based mostly on etymological i.e. morphological instead of phonetic principles. English written syllables therefore do not correspond to the actually spoken syllables of the living language. Phonotactic rules determine which sounds are allowed or disallowed in each part of the syllable. English language, English allows very complicated syllables; syllables may begin with up to three consonants (as in ''strength''), and occasionally end with as many as five (as in ''angsts'', pronounced [æŋsts]). (Some dialects of English pronounce ''strengths'' with a four-consonant onset, and ''angsts'' with a five-consonant coda: [stʃɹɛŋkθ] and [æŋksts] respectively.) Many other languages are much more restricted; Japanese language, Japanese, for example, only allows and a chroneme in a coda, and theoretically has no consonant clusters at all, as the onset is composed of at most one consonant. The linking of a word-final consonant to a vowel beginning the word immediately following it forms a regular part of the phonetics of some languages, including Spanish, Hungarian, and Turkish. Thus, in Spanish, the phrase ('the men') is pronounced , Hungarian ('the human') as , and Turkish ('I hated it') as . In Italian, a final sound can be moved to the next syllable in enchainement, sometimes with a gemination: e.g., ('I've never had any of them') is broken into syllables as and ('I go there and she does as well') is realized as . A related phenomenon, called consonant mutation, is found in the Celtic languages like Irish and Welsh, whereby unwritten (but historical) final consonants affect the initial consonant of the following word.


Ambisyllabicity

There can be disagreement about the location of some divisions between syllables in spoken language. The problems of dealing with such cases have been most commonly discussed with relation to English. In the case of a word such as ''hurry'', the division may be or , neither of which seems a satisfactory analysis for a non-rhotic accent such as RP (British English): results in a syllable-final , which is not normally found, while gives a syllable-final short stressed vowel, which is also non-occurring. Arguments can be made in favour of one solution or the other: A general rule has been proposed that states that "Subject to certain conditions ..., consonants are syllabified with the more strongly stressed of two flanking syllables", while many other phonologists prefer to divide syllables with the consonant or consonants attached to the following syllable wherever possible. However, an alternative that has received some support is to treat an intervocalic consonant as ''ambisyllabic'', i.e. belonging both to the preceding and to the following syllable: . This is discussed in more detail in .


Onset

The onset (also known as anlaut) is the consonant sound or sounds at the beginning of a syllable, occurring before the #Nucleus, nucleus. Most syllables have an onset. Syllables without an onset may be said to have an ''empty'' or ''zero (linguistics), zero onset'' – that is, nothing where the onset would be.


Onset cluster

Some languages restrict onsets to be only a single consonant, while others allow multiconsonant onsets according to various rules. For example, in English, onsets such as ''pr-'', ''pl-'' and ''tr-'' are possible but ''tl-'' is not, and ''sk-'' is possible but ''ks-'' is not. In Greek language, Greek, however, both ''ks-'' and ''tl-'' are possible onsets, while contrarily in Classical Arabic no multiconsonant onsets are allowed at all.


Null onset

Some languages forbid null onsets. In these languages, words beginning in a vowel, like the English word ''at'', are impossible. This is less strange than it may appear at first, as most such languages allow syllables to begin with a phonemic glottal stop (the sound in the middle of English ''uh-oh'' or, in some dialects, the double T in ''button'', represented in the International Phonetic Alphabet, IPA as ). In English, a word that begins with a vowel may be pronounced with an epenthesis, epenthetic glottal stop when following a pause, though the glottal stop may not be a phoneme in the language. Few languages make a phonemic distinction between a word beginning with a vowel and a word beginning with a glottal stop followed by a vowel, since the distinction will generally only be audible following another word. However, Maltese language, Maltese and some Polynesian languages do make such a distinction, as in Hawaiian language, Hawaiian ('fire') and / ← ('tuna') and Maltese ← Arabic and Maltese ← Arabic . Ashkenazi Hebrew, Ashkenazi and Sephardi Hebrew may commonly ignore , and , and Arabic forbid empty onsets. The names ''Israel'', ''Abel'', ''Abraham'', ''Omar'', ''Abdullah'', and ''Iraq'' appear not to have onsets in the first syllable, but in the original Hebrew and Arabic forms they actually begin with various consonants: the semivowel in , the glottal fricative in , the glottal stop in , or the pharyngeal fricative in , , and . Conversely, the Arrernte language of central Australia may prohibit onsets altogether; if so, all syllables have the underlying representation, underlying shape VC(C). The difference between a syllable with a null onset and one beginning with a glottal stop is often purely a difference of phonology, phonological analysis, rather than the actual pronunciation of the syllable. In some cases, the pronunciation of a (putatively) vowel-initial word when following another word – particularly, whether or not a glottal stop is inserted – indicates whether the word should be considered to have a null onset. For example, many Romance languages such as Spanish language, Spanish never insert such a glottal stop, while English language, English does so only some of the time, depending on factors such as conversation speed; in both cases, this suggests that the words in question are truly vowel-initial. But there are exceptions here, too. For example, standard German language, German (excluding many southern accents) and Arabic language, Arabic both require that a glottal stop be inserted between a word and a following, putatively vowel-initial word. Yet such words are perceived to begin with a vowel in German but a glottal stop in Arabic. The reason for this has to do with other properties of the two languages. For example, a glottal stop does not occur in other situations in German, e.g. before a consonant or at the end of word. On the other hand, in Arabic, not only does a glottal stop occur in such situations (e.g. Classical "he asked", "opinion", "light"), but it occurs in alternations that are clearly indicative of its phonemic status (cf. Classical "writer" vs. /mak "written", "eater" vs. "eaten"). In other words, while the glottal stop is predictable in German (inserted only if a stressed syllable would otherwise begin with a vowel), the same sound is a regular consonantal phoneme in Arabic. The status of this consonant in the respective writing systems corresponds to this difference: there is no reflex of the glottal stop in German orthography, but there is a letter in the Arabic alphabet (Hamza (ء)). The writing system of a language may not correspond with the phonological analysis of the language in terms of its handling of (potentially) null onsets. For example, in some languages written in the Latin alphabet, an initial glottal stop is left unwritten (see the German example); on the other hand, some languages written using non-Latin alphabets such as abjads and abugidas have a special zero consonant to represent a null onset. As an example, in Hangul, the alphabet of the Korean language, a null onset is represented with ㅇ at the left or top section of a grapheme, as in "station", pronounced ''yeok'', where the diphthong ''yeo'' is the nucleus and ''k'' is the coda.


Nucleus

The ''nucleus'' is usually the vowel in the middle of a syllable. Generally, every syllable requires a nucleus (sometimes called the ''peak''), and the minimal syllable consists only of a nucleus, as in the English words "eye" or "owe". The syllable nucleus is usually a vowel, in the form of a monophthong, diphthong, or triphthong, but sometimes is a syllabic consonant. In most Germanic languages, tenseness, lax vowels can occur only in closed syllables. Therefore, these vowels are also called Checked and free vowels, checked vowels, as opposed to the tense vowels that are called ''free vowels'' because they can occur even in open syllables.


Consonant nucleus

The notion of syllable is challenged by languages that allow long strings of obstruents without any intervening vowel or sonorant. By far the most common syllabic consonants are sonorants like , , , or , as in English ''bottle'', ''church'' (in rhotic accents), ''rhythm'', ''button'' and ''lock n key''. However, English allows syllabic obstruents in a few para-verbal Onomatopoeia, onomatopoeic utterances such as ''shh'' (used to command silence) and ''psst'' (used to attract attention). All of these have been analyzed as phonemically syllabic. Obstruent-only syllables also occur phonetically in some prosodic situations when unstressed vowels elide between obstruents, as in ''potato'' and ''today'' , which do not change in their number of syllables despite losing a syllabic nucleus. A few languages have so-called ''syllabic fricatives'', also known as ''fricative vowels'', at the phonemic level. (In the context of Standard Chinese phonology, Chinese phonology, the related but non-synonymous term ''apical vowel'' is commonly used.) Mandarin Chinese is famous for having such sounds in at least some of its dialects, for example the pinyin syllables ''sī shī rī'', usually pronounced , respectively. Though, like the nucleus of rhotic English ''church'', there is debate over whether these nuclei are consonants or vowels. Languages of the northwest coast of North America, including Salishan languages, Salishan, Wakashan languages, Wakashan and Chinookan languages, Chinookan languages, allow stop consonants and voiceless fricatives as syllables at the phonemic level, in even the most careful enunciation. An example is Chinook 'those two women are coming this way out of the water'. Linguists have analyzed this situation in various ways, some arguing that such syllables have no nucleus at all and some arguing that the concept of "syllable" cannot clearly be applied at all to these languages. Other examples: ; Nuxálk language, Nuxálk (Bella Coola) : 'you spat on me' : 'he arrived' : 'he had in his possession a bunchberry plant' : 'seal blubber' In Bagemihl's survey of previous analyses, he finds that the Bella Coola word 'he arrived' would have been parsed into 0, 2, 3, 5, or 6 syllables depending on which analysis is used. One analysis would consider all vowel and consonant segments as syllable nuclei, another would consider only a small subset (fricatives or sibilants) as nuclei candidates, and another would simply deny the existence of syllables completely. However, when working with recordings rather than transcriptions, the syllables can be obvious in such languages, and native speakers have strong intuitions as to what the syllables are. This type of phenomenon has also been reported in Berber languages (such as Indlawn Shilha language, Tashlhiyt Berber), Mon–Khmer languages (such as Semai language, Semai, Temiar language, Temiar, Khmu language, Khmu) and the Ōgami dialect of Miyako language, Miyako, a Ryukyuan languages, Ryukyuan language. ; Indlawn Tashlhiyt Berber : 'you sprained it and then gave it' : 'rot' (imperf.) ; Semai : 'short, fat arms'


Coda

The coda (also known as auslaut) comprises the consonant sounds of a syllable that follow the #Nucleus, nucleus. The sequence of nucleus and coda is called a #Rime, rime. Some syllables consist of only a nucleus, only an onset and a nucleus with no coda, or only a nucleus and coda with no onset. The phonotactics of many languages forbid syllable codas. Examples are Swahili language, Swahili and Hawaiian language, Hawaiian. In others, codas are restricted to a small subset of the consonants that appear in onset position. At a phonemic level in Japanese language, Japanese, for example, a coda may only be a nasal (homorganic with any following consonant) or, in the middle of a word, gemination of the following consonant. (On a phonetic level, other codas occur due to elision of /i/ and /u/.) In other languages, nearly any consonant allowed as an onset is also allowed in the coda, even consonant cluster, clusters of consonants. In English, for example, all onset consonants except are allowed as syllable codas. If the coda consists of a consonant cluster, the sonority typically decreases from first to last, as in the English word ''help''. This is called the sonority hierarchy (or sonority scale). English onset and coda clusters are therefore different. The onset in ''strengths'' does not appear as a coda in any English word. However, some clusters do occur as both onsets and codas, such as in ''stardust''. The sonority hierarchy is more strict in some languages and less strict in others.


Open and closed

A coda-less syllable of the form V, CV, CCV, etc. (V = vowel, C = consonant) is called an open syllable or free syllable, while a syllable that has a coda (VC, CVC, CVCC, etc.) is called a closed syllable or checked syllable. They have nothing to do with open vowel, open and close vowels, but are defined according to the phoneme that ends the syllable: a vowel (open syllable) or a consonant (closed syllable). Almost all languages allow open syllables, but some, such as Hawaiian language, Hawaiian, do not have closed syllables. When a syllable is not the last syllable in a word, the nucleus normally must be followed by two consonants in order for the syllable to be closed. This is because a single following consonant is typically considered the onset of the following syllable. For example, Spanish ("to marry") is composed of an open syllable followed by a closed syllable (''ca-sar''), whereas "to get tired" is composed of two closed syllables (''can-sar''). When a geminate (double) consonant occurs, the syllable boundary occurs in the middle, e.g. Italian "cream" (''pan-na''); cf. Italian "bread" (''pa-ne''). English words may consist of a single closed syllable, with nucleus denoted by ν, and coda denoted by κ: *i''n'': ν = , κ = *cu''p'': ν = , κ = *ta''ll'': ν = , κ = *mi''lk'': ν = , κ = *ti''nts'': ν = , κ = *fi''fths'': ν = , κ = *si''xths'': ν = , κ = *twe''lfths'': ν = , κ = *stre''ngths'': ν = , κ = English words may also consist of a single open syllable, ending in a nucleus, without a coda: *''glue'', ν = *''pie'', ν = *''though'', ν = *''boy'', ν = A list of examples of syllable codas in English is found at English phonology#Coda.


Null coda

Some languages, such as Hawaiian phonology#Phonotactics, Hawaiian, forbid codas, so that all syllables are open.


Suprasegmental features

The domain of Suprasegmental, suprasegmental features is the syllable (or some larger unit), but not a specific sound. That is to say, these features may effect more than a single segment, and possibly all segments of a syllable: *Lexical stress, Stress *Tone (linguistics), Tone * *Palatalization (phonetics)#Types, Suprasegmental palatalization Sometimes syllable length is also counted as a suprasegmental feature; for example, in some Germanic languages, long vowels may only exist with short consonants and vice versa. However, syllables can be analyzed as compositions of long and short phonemes, as in Finnish and Japanese, where consonant gemination and vowel length are independent.


Tone

In most languages, the pitch or pitch contour in which a syllable is pronounced conveys shades of meaning such as emphasis or surprise, or distinguishes a statement from a question. In tonal languages, however, the pitch affects the basic lexical meaning (e.g. "cat" vs. "dog") or grammatical meaning (e.g. past vs. present). In some languages, only the pitch itself (e.g. high vs. low) has this effect, while in others, especially East Asian languages such as Chinese language, Chinese, Thai language, Thai or Vietnamese language, Vietnamese, the shape or contour (e.g. level vs. rising vs. falling) also needs to be distinguished.


Accent

Syllable structure often interacts with stress or pitch accent. In Latin, for example, stress is regularly determined by syllable weight, a syllable counting as heavy if it has at least one of the following: * a long vowel in its Syllable nucleus, nucleus * a diphthong in its nucleus * one or more Syllable coda, codas In each case the syllable is considered to have two Mora (linguistics), morae. The first syllable of a word is the initial syllable and the last syllable is the final syllable. In languages accented on one of the last three syllables, the last syllable is called the Ultima (linguistics), ultima, the next-to-last is called the penult, and the third syllable from the end is called the antepenult. These terms come from Latin ' "last", ' "almost last", and ' "before almost last". In Ancient Greek, there are three Greek diacritics, accent marks (acute, circumflex, and grave), and terms were used to describe words based on the position and type of accent. Some of these terms are used in the description of other languages.


History

Guilhem Molinier, a member of the Consistori del Gay Saber, which was the first literary academy in the world and held the Floral Games to award the best troubadour with the ''violeta d'aur'' top prize, gave a definition of the syllable in his ''Leys d'amor'' (1328–1337), a book aimed at regulating then-flourishing Occitan language, Occitan poetry:


See also

*English phonology#Phonotactics. Covers syllable structure in English. *Entering tone *International Phonetic Alphabet#Suprasegmentals, IPA symbols for syllables *Line (poetry) *List of the longest English words with one syllable *Minor syllable *Mora (linguistics) *Phonology *Pitch accent *Stress (linguistics) *Syllabary writing system *Syllabic consonant *Syllabification *Syllable (computing) *Timing (linguistics)


References


Sources and recommended reading

* * * * * * *


External links


Syllable Dictionary: Look up the number of syllables in a word. Learn to divide into syllables. Hear it pronounced.

Do syllables have internal structure? What is their status in phonology? CUNY Phonology Forum

Syllable Word Counter A comprehensive database of words and their syllables

Syllable drill. Listen to syllables and select its representation in Latin letters

Syllable counter: Count the number of syllables for any word or sentence.
{{Authority control Linguistic units Phonotactics Phonology