Crow language
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Crow ( native name: ''Apsáalooke'' ) is a Missouri Valley Siouan language spoken primarily by the
Crow Nation The Crow, whose autonym is Apsáalooke (), also spelled Absaroka, are Native Americans living primarily in southern Montana. Today, the Crow people have a federally recognized tribe, the Crow Tribe of Montana, with an Indian reservation loc ...
in present-day southeastern
Montana Montana () is a state in the Mountain West division of the Western United States. It is bordered by Idaho to the west, North Dakota and South Dakota to the east, Wyoming to the south, and the Canadian provinces of Alberta, British Columb ...
. The word, ''Apsáalooke,'' translates to "children of the raven." It is one of the larger populations of American Indian languages with 2,480 speakers according to the 1990 US
Census A census is the procedure of systematically acquiring, recording and calculating information about the members of a given population. This term is used mostly in connection with national population and housing censuses; other common censuses inc ...
.Ethnologue


Dialects

Crow is closely related to
Hidatsa The Hidatsa are a Siouan people. They are enrolled in the federally recognized Three Affiliated Tribes of the Fort Berthold Reservation in North Dakota. Their language is related to that of the Crow, and they are sometimes considered a paren ...
spoken by the
Hidatsa The Hidatsa are a Siouan people. They are enrolled in the federally recognized Three Affiliated Tribes of the Fort Berthold Reservation in North Dakota. Their language is related to that of the Crow, and they are sometimes considered a paren ...
tribe of the Dakotas; the two languages are the only members of the Missouri Valley Siouan family.Silver and Miller 1997: 367.Graczyk, 2007: 2 Despite their similarities, Crow and Hidatsa are not mutually intelligible.


Status

According to Ethnologue with figures from 1998, 77% of
Crow people The Crow, whose autonym is Apsáalooke (), also spelled Absaroka, are Native Americans living primarily in southern Montana. Today, the Crow people have a federally recognized tribe, the Crow Tribe of Montana, with an Indian reservation loc ...
over 66 years old speak the language; "some" parents and older adults, "few" high school students and "no pre-schoolers" speak Crow. 80% of the
Crow Nation The Crow, whose autonym is Apsáalooke (), also spelled Absaroka, are Native Americans living primarily in southern Montana. Today, the Crow people have a federally recognized tribe, the Crow Tribe of Montana, with an Indian reservation loc ...
prefers to speak in English. The language was defined as "definitely endangered" by UNESCO as of 2012. However, R. Graczyk claims in his ''A Grammar of Crow'' (2007) that " like many other native languages of North America in general, and the northern plain in particular, the Crow language still exhibits considerable vitality: there are fluent speakers of all ages, and at least some children are still acquiring Crow as their first language." Many of the younger population who do not speak Crow are able to understand it. Almost all of those who do speak Crow are also bilingual in English. Graczyk cites the reservation community as the reason for both the high level of bilingual Crow-English speakers and the continued use and prevalence of the Crow language. Daily contact with non–American Indians on the reservation for over one hundred years has led to high usage of English. Traditional culture within the community, however, has preserved the language via religious ceremonies and the traditional clan system. Currently, most speakers of Crow are 30 and older but a few younger speakers are learning it. There are increased efforts for children to learn Crow as their first language and many do on the Crow Reservation of
Montana Montana () is a state in the Mountain West division of the Western United States. It is bordered by Idaho to the west, North Dakota and South Dakota to the east, Wyoming to the south, and the Canadian provinces of Alberta, British Columb ...
, particularly through a Crow language immersion school that was sponsored in 2012. Development for the language includes a Crow language dictionary and portions of the
Bible The Bible (from Koine Greek , , 'the books') is a collection of religious texts or scriptures that are held to be sacred in Christianity Christianity is an Abrahamic monotheistic religion based on the life and teachings of Jesus ...
published from 1980-2007. The current literacy rate is around 1-5% for first language speakers and 75-100% for second language learners. Teens are immersed in Crow at the Apsaalooke language camp sponsored by the Crow Nation.


Classification

Crow is closely related to
Hidatsa The Hidatsa are a Siouan people. They are enrolled in the federally recognized Three Affiliated Tribes of the Fort Berthold Reservation in North Dakota. Their language is related to that of the Crow, and they are sometimes considered a paren ...
spoken by the
Hidatsa The Hidatsa are a Siouan people. They are enrolled in the federally recognized Three Affiliated Tribes of the Fort Berthold Reservation in North Dakota. Their language is related to that of the Crow, and they are sometimes considered a paren ...
tribe of the Dakotas; the two languages are the only members of the Missouri Valley Siouan family. The ancestor of Crow-Hidatsa may have constituted the initial split from Proto-Siouan. Crow and
Hidatsa The Hidatsa are a Siouan people. They are enrolled in the federally recognized Three Affiliated Tribes of the Fort Berthold Reservation in North Dakota. Their language is related to that of the Crow, and they are sometimes considered a paren ...
are not mutually intelligible, however the two languages share many phonological features, cognates and have similar morphologies and syntax. The split between Crow and
Hidatsa The Hidatsa are a Siouan people. They are enrolled in the federally recognized Three Affiliated Tribes of the Fort Berthold Reservation in North Dakota. Their language is related to that of the Crow, and they are sometimes considered a paren ...
may have occurred between 300 and 800 years ago.Hollow and Parks, 1980: 68-97.


Phonology


Vowels

There are five distinct vowels in Crow, which occur either long or short with the exception of the mid vowels. There is also a marginal diphthong ''ea'' that only occurs in two native Crow stems: ''déaxa'' 'clear' and ''béaxa'' 'intermittent'.


Consonants

Crow has a very sparse consonant inventory, much like many other languages of the
Great Plains The Great Plains (french: Grandes Plaines), sometimes simply "the Plains", is a broad expanse of flatland in North America. It is located west of the Mississippi River and east of the Rocky Mountains, much of it covered in prairie, steppe, a ...
. Stops are aspirated word-initially, word-finally, when geminated (e.g. and when following another stop (e.g.
In phonetics, aspiration is the strong burst of breath that accompanies either the release or, in the case of preaspiration, the closure of some obstruents. In English, aspirated consonants are allophones in complementary distribution with t ...
. Stops in a consonant cluster with ''h'' as the initial radical (''hp'', ''ht'', ''hk'') are unaspirated and lax. Gemination in stops only occurs intervocalically. Intervocalic single, nongeminating stops are lax, unaspirated, and generally voiced. The difference between voiced stops ''b'' and ''d'' (allophones of ''m'' and ''n'') and voiceless stops is hardly discernible when following a fricative, since both are unaspirated and lax. The phoneme ''k'' has a palatalized allophone ʲthat occurs after ''i'', ''e'', ''ch'' and ''sh'', often word-finally.
Fricative A fricative is a consonant produced by forcing air through a narrow channel made by placing two articulators close together. These may be the lower lip against the upper teeth, in the case of ; the back of the tongue against the soft palate in ...
s are tense; they are only lax when intervocalic. Palatal ''sh'' is often voiced intervocalically; ''s'' is sometimes voiced intervocalically; ''x'' is never voiced. The alveolar fricative /s/ has an optional allophone /h/ in phrase-initial position: *''sáapa'' "what" > áapa*''sapée'' "who" > apée
Sonorant In phonetics and phonology, a sonorant or resonant is a speech sound that is produced with continuous, non-turbulent airflow in the vocal tract; these are the manners of articulation that are most often voiced in the world's languages. Vowels ar ...
s voiced /m/ and /n/ have three allophones: ''w'' and ''l'' intervocalically, ''b'' and ''d'' word initially and following an obstruent, and ''m'' and ''n'' in all other conditions. In conservative speech, ''l'' is realized as a tapped ''r'', however in general cases it is realized as ''l'', perhaps due in part to the influence of English. Word initially, ''b'' is optional for /m/, though ''b'' is more commonly realized. The glottal sonorant /h/ assimilates to the nasality of the following segment, but retains its voicelessness. When following ''i'' or ''e'' or preceding ''ch'', /h/ may be realized as an alveopalatal fricative.


Structure

Vowel sequences across
morpheme A morpheme is the smallest meaningful Constituent (linguistics), constituent of a linguistic expression. The field of linguistics, linguistic study dedicated to morphemes is called morphology (linguistics), morphology. In English, morphemes are ...
boundaries can be quite varied, but short vowels cannot appear alone in the morpheme: V:V (long+short), V:V: (long+long) and diphthong+V (short). Word finally, only ''a'' (in a diphthong), ''o'', and ''u'' (allomorphs of the plural suffix) can occur after a long vowel. A wide variety of
consonant cluster In linguistics, a consonant cluster, consonant sequence or consonant compound, is a group of consonants which have no intervening vowel. In English, for example, the groups and are consonant clusters in the word ''splits''. In the education fie ...
s can occur in Crow. All consonants except for /h/ can be
geminated In phonetics and phonology, gemination (), or consonant lengthening (from Latin 'doubling', itself from '' gemini'' 'twins'), is an articulation of a consonant for a longer period of time than that of a singleton consonant. It is distinct fr ...
. Voiced labials and dentals (phonemic ''m'' and ''n'', allophones ''b'', ''m'', ''w'' and ''d'', ''n'', ''l'') are resistant to clustering. Because they only occur intervocallically, ''l'' and ''w'' do not occur in clusters. The plosive
allophone In phonology, an allophone (; from the Greek , , 'other' and , , 'voice, sound') is a set of multiple possible spoken soundsor ''phones''or signs used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language. For example, in English, (as in '' ...
s ''b'' and ''d'' only occur in clusters as the second consonant and only at morpheme boundaries. The nasal allophones ''m'' and ''n'' can only occur with each other with the exception of ''nm'', or occur with ''h'' at a morpheme boundary. Clusters in general occur at morphemic boundaries. Some morphemic constraints: *A word begins either with a V (long or short) or a single C; no word-initial consonant clusters *Consonant clusters only occur word-internally; exception: ''sht'' as a single morpheme is an emphatic sentence-final declarative marker. *A word can end in any C except for ''p'' and ''x''; ''ch'' only occurs in one word (''iach'') as a plural demonstrative *All lexical nouns and verb stems end in a vowel *Generally, nonderived noun and verb stems consist of between 1-4 syllables. *Only V: or diphthongs occur in one-syllable word


Stress

Stress Stress may refer to: Science and medicine * Stress (biology), an organism's response to a stressor such as an environmental condition * Stress (linguistics), relative emphasis or prominence given to a syllable in a word, or to a word in a phrase ...
in Crow is phonemic. The position of the stress in the stem is determined lexically. Virtually all noun and verb stems have an inherent stress. In word initial syllables, accented short vowels in a word initial syllable are generally followed by a consonant cluster, while accented long vowels are generally followed by a single consonant. Stress can fall on short vowels as well as long vowels and may fall on either mora of a long vowel. With diphthongs, either the long vowel or the offglide may bear the stress. Stress helps predict the tones of all the vowels in a word: stressed vowels are high in pitch; all vowels following the stressed vowel are low in pitch; all short vowels preceding the stressed vowel are low in pitch; all long vowels preceding the stressed vowel are high in pitch; short vowels occurring between a long vowel and the accented vowel assimilate to a high pitch. In words composed of more than one morpheme, there are several rules (with a few exceptions) to determine the placement of the stress: *If the first stressed morpheme is stressed anywhere except for the final mora of a stem-final vowel, the subsequent morpheme is unstressed. *If the first stressed morpheme has its accent of the stem-final vowel mora, that morpheme loses its stress. *If the morpheme following the first stress lacks lexical stress, the stress remains on the first morpheme. *If a stress stem-final vowel is deleted when the following morpheme lacks lexical stress, the stress is transferred to the preceding vowel mora of the deleted vowel. Exceptions: *A few stems with final falling accent have long high stress for the purposes of word formation. *The punctual aspectual marker ''áhi'' overrides the regular word accent - it is always accented *The exclamative sentence-final marker ''wík'' is stressed in addition to the stress of the stem to which it is combined. Vowel morae that occur between the first stress and the exclamative suffix are low in pitch.


Phonological processes

Phonological processes in Crow include: *short vowel deletion: stem-final short vowels are deleted at a morpheme boundary unless a three-consonant cluster or a nasal plus voiceless obstruent would occur. Stem-final vowels do not delete before ''dak'', the coordinate noun-phrase conjunction. Sentence-final evidential suffixes also do not cause the final short vowel to be deleted. *nasal assimilation: ''n'' assimilates to ''m'' in a cluster; ''nm'' clusters do not occur. *sibilant assimilation: alveolar ''s'' and ''ss'' are realized as /sh/ at morpheme boundaries before all consonants except ''x'' and ''s''. *vowel neutralization: word-finally, stem-final short vowels ''i'', ''a'' and ''u'' are neutralized to their corresponding mid nonround or round vowel: ''i'', ''a'' become ''e''; ''u'' becomes ''o''. *identical vowel reduction: with suffixes beginning with ''a'', sequences of 3-4 identical vowel morae are reduced to two (''aa-a'' and ''aa-aa'' are reduced to ''aa''); exceptions are compounds and prefixes. *long vowel reduction before ''h'': long vowels shorten before ''h'' in a syllable coda. *final schwa deletion: the final schwa of a diphthong is deleted before suffixes beginning with ''a'' and before the plural; before other vowels, it is otherwise retained. *palatal-dental alternation: stem-final ''ch'' and ''t'' are complementary; ''t'' occurs before ''a''-initial suffixes and plural ''u'', and ''ch'' everywhere else. This relations holds parallel for ''š'' - ''s''; and geminates ''čč'' and ''šš''. The ''č'' and ''š'' alternates occur before nonlow vowels, whereas ''t'' and ''s'' occur before low vowels. There are, however, a few exceptions to this complementary relationship, therefore these phonemes cannot be considered as allophones. *palatal-velar alternation: there is a lexically conditioned ''č'' to ''k'' alternation; ''k'' occurs before the plural and before suffixes beginning with ''a'', not producing ''t''. *stem ablaut: lexically conditioned alternation affecting stem-final long vowels triggered by the plural morphemes, the imperative, and ''a''-initial suffixes. (''ii'' to ''aa'' ablaut; ''ee'' to ''ii'' ablaut; ''ee'' to ''aa'' ablaut.


Morphology

Crow is a
polysynthetic language In linguistic typology, polysynthetic languages, formerly holophrastic languages, are highly synthetic languages, i.e. languages in which words are composed of many morphemes (word parts that have independent meaning but may or may not be able ...
.


Nominal morphology

Basic stems consist of one to four syllables (with four being rare) and always end in a vowel. Monosyllabic stems have long vowels or diphthongs, e.g., ''bií'', 'stone, rock'; ''bía'', 'woman'. The vast majority of nouns in Crow are derived stems. Derivational processes in nominal morphology include affixation and compounding.


Suffixes

An exhaustive list of nominal suffixes: * - 'approximative': follows a stem-final short vowel, follows a stem-final long vowel. Marks resemblance or similarity: 'kind of, sort of, like, (temporal) around the time of.' * - 'real, true; very' * - 'diminutive': Can add the diminutive meaning 'small, little' or the endearing, affectionate meaning 'dear' according to the semantics of the noun. * - 'sportive, imitative': Marks resemblance or imitation. * - 'real, genuine': Marks an object's reality, its genuineness. Often reduced to ''táali''. * - 'here and there': Most commonly occurs with verbs, though occasionally is attached to nouns. * - 'even': Marks concessive subordinate clauses as 'although, even though, even if.' Also occurs as a noun suffix glossed as 'even.' ''Htaa'' is a rare suffix that combines with the bare nominal stem of the noun.


Prefixes

Prefixes will render a relative clause into a derived noun. * - 'agent nominalizer': creates agentive nouns (ex. 'singer', 'dancer') from active verbs or verbs plus incorporated objects. * - 'locative, temporal, or manner nominalizer': 'where, when, how' derived from verbs or verbs plus incorporated nouns. In some cases, ''ala'' may follow the noun creating a lexicalized relative clause. * - 'indefinite nominalizer': Derived from stative verbs, inalienably possessed nouns plus stative verbs, active transitive verbs, and from active intransitive verbs. * - 'instrumental nominalizer': Derived from active transitive and intransitive verbs, and from transitive verbs plus incorporated nouns. * - 'depossessivizer': Allows an inalienably possessed noun to occur without a possessor.


Compounding

There are two basic types of compounding in Crow: noun-noun compounds and noun-verb compounds. Noun-noun compounds often involve a whole-part relationship: the first noun refers to the whole and the second to the part. Members of the compound may also be themselves compounds or derived nouns. Noun-verb compounds consist of a noun plus a stative verb. There are a number of select exceptions.


Possession

Nouns are classified as either inalienably or alienably possessed, according to which possessive markers they occur with. Inalienably possessed nouns are those that are inherently possessed or nondetachable associations, specifically body parts and family members, opposed to alienably possessed nouns whose entity is not inherently possessed. This rule is not absolute as some body parts and kin nouns can be considered alienable and some nouns with close associations to its possessor (i.e., 'his house', 'her dog') can be considered inalienable. The affixed possession paradigm for inalienable and alienable possessives can be derived. The alienable possessives only use the first consonant of the alienable prefixes and do not mark the possessor when the prefix begins with a vowel. The final suffix transforms into a diphthong from /-o/.


Personal names

Personal names constitute a distinct morphological class of nouns in Crow. They are marked with the definite determiner suffix /sh/, which attaches to the stem rather than to the citation form.


Pronouns

Crow has three pronominal forms: bound; emphatic and contrastive; and interrogative-indefinite pronouns. With the first two types, there is a correlation between morphology and syntax. Argument pronouns are generally bound whereas emphatic and contrastive pronouns are generally independent. Bound pronominals function as direct and
oblique argument In linguistics, an argument is an expression that helps complete the meaning of a predicate, the latter referring in this context to a main verb and its auxiliaries. In this regard, the ''complement'' is a closely related concept. Most predicates ...
s. *A-set pronominals mark only subjects of active verbs, both transitive and intransitive. *B-set pronominals mark subjects of stative verbs, direct objects, and objects of postpositions. Bound Pronominal Stems:


Verbal morphology

Verbal derivational morphology is composed of prefixes, suffixes, one infix (''chi'', 'again; possessive reflexive') and reduplication, which expresses an "iterative, distributive, or intensive sense to the meaning of the stem."Graczyk, 2007: 104.


Active–stative verbs

The morphological verb classes in Crow mirror a semantic distinction: Crow is an active–stative language, meaning that the subject of an active verb is treated differently than the subject of a stative verb. Active verbs and stative verbs are marked with distinct sets of pronominal affixes: the "A-set" for active verbs and the "B-set" for stative verbs. Active verbs may have one, two, or three arguments (making them respectively intransitive, transitive, or ditransitive). An intransitive verb takes a subject (SV), a transitive verb takes a subject and an object (SOV) and a ditransitive verb takes a subject and two objects (SO1O2V). In a relative clause built on an active verb, when the subject of the verb is the head of the relative clause and it is an animate noun phrase, it is marked by ''ak''. Stative verbs may have zero (impersonal), one, or two arguments. In a relative clause, the subject of a stative verb is marked with ''m'' or in elevated discourse, ''dak''. There may also be an absence of marking on the head noun where the entire relative clause is marked with the indefinite nonspecific determiner ''m''.


Verb chain

Crow has a fairly complex ordering of verb phrase constituents. The following table demonstrates simple constructions of active-state intransitive and transitive verbs based on the first person. *1-4) Active ("A-Set") pronominals in Crow are very diverse coming in many different forms based on the based bound form. They are patterned by a certain list of lexical and phonological factors, such as the ''dú(u) - by hand'' pattern which results in a 1sg ''bu'' and a 2sg ''di'', or the ''dá(a) - by mouth'' pattern which results in a 1sg ''ba'' and a 2sg ''da''. *2) For Active-Intransitive Causative Verbs, 1-2 person singular causitive (rank 10) is marked by ''aa'' as in chart, 1-2 person plural is marked by ''uu'', 3rd person singular is marked by ''ee'' or ''a'' determined lexically, and 3rd person plural is marked by either ''uu'', ''o'', or ''iio'' determined lexically. *4) For Active-Transitive Causative Verbs, the causative transitive verb subject is marked by ''wa'' in the first person, ''la'' in the second person and ''0'' in the third person. The Causative affixes are ''hc'' (singular) and ''hk'' (plural). *Mood in Crow is expressed by a variety of post-positionals. The standard indicative morpheme is ''k''. The verb chain constituents are, of course, much more complicated. Following is a concise list of the rank ordering of each type element: *Prefixes: **I: Adverbial proclitics: **II: B-set pronominal elements **III: A-set pronominal elements **IV: Locative prefixes **V: Instrumental prefixes *Stem: **VI: Stem modification - reduplication or prefixation and infixation of ''chi/ku'' "again" *Suffixes **VII: Derivational suffixes **VIII: Punctual ''áhi'' **IX: Continuative, modal, or benefactive auxiliary **X: Habitual ''i'' **XI: Plural **XII: Subordinate clause markers ***a. Speech act and evidential markers ***b. Switch reference markers ***c. Subordinate clause markers ***d. Clauses without final markers **XIII: Negative ''ssaa''


Syntax

Crow is a subject–object–verb ( SOV) language; it is a verb-final and head marking. In noun phrases, the order is possessor–possessum, with the person marker of the possessor identified by a prefix to the possessum. Subordinate clauses precede matrix clauses, and are marked by a suffixed clause-final marker. Relative clauses are internally headed. Crow has postpositional phrases, with the postposition often occurring as a prefix to the following verb. There is no distinct category of adjectives; instead, stative verbs function as noun phrase modifiers. Crow is an active–stative language, with verbs divided into two classes, active (both transitive and intransitive) and stative, largely on semantic grounds. This is also often called a "split intransitive" language.


Noun-phrase syntax

An analysis of Crow noun phrase syntax under
generative grammar Generative grammar, or generativism , is a linguistic theory that regards linguistics as the study of a hypothesised innate grammatical structure. It is a biological or biologistic modification of earlier structuralist theories of linguisti ...
has yielded the following rules: *a. NP → N' (DET) *b. ** i. N' → N ** ii. N' → ...N' head... (relative clause) ** iii. N' → NP N' (genitive/possessive) ** iv. N' → PP N' (PP modifier) *c. Q → DP Q (quantifier phrase) *d. DP → DEM NP (demonstrative phrase) *e. NP → NP NP (appositive) *f. NP → S (COMP) (nominalization) *g. ** i. NP → (NP CONJ)^n (coordinate NP with ''dak'') ** ii. NP → (N' CONJ)^n DET (coordinate N' with ''xxo'') There are two phrases that are subordinate to the NP (noun-phrase): (1) the DP (demonstrative phrase) and (2) the QP (quantifier phrase). A noun phrase can be marked as definite or indefinite by a suffixed
determiner A determiner, also called determinative ( abbreviated ), is a word, phrase, or affix that occurs together with a noun or noun phrase and generally serves to express the reference of that noun or noun phrase in the context. That is, a determine ...
(DET). The definite suffix is /-sh/ and the indefinite suffix is /-m/. **''iisáakshee-sh'' (definite) ***'the young man' **''bía-m'' (indefinite) ***'a woman' The determiner suffix is attached to the final word of the noun phrase, not just the agentive noun. Relative Clauses: N' → ...N' head... Genitive Clauses: N' → NP N' Postpositional Phrases: N' → PP N' Quantifier Phrases: Q → DP Q There are two classes of quantifiers that are distinguished syntactically. The first class heads a quantifier phrases that takes a demonstrative (or in its stead, a noun phrase) as its complement: ''xaxúa''. The second class is a stative verb that may function as a nominal modifier. This class includes: ''ahú'' 'many, much', ''hawa'' 'some', ''kooshtá'' 'few', ''sáawi'' 'how many, so many, some', and the numerals. This class may also be followed by a determiner. They may also function as clausal predicates. Demonstrative Phrases: Q → DP Q Demonstratives are deictic words; in Crow, they occur phrase-initially. They can also cooccur with determiners (ex. 'this the horse'). Appositives: NP → NP NP /ko/ (demonstrative) and /kon/ (appositive) are used to modify each other.


Relative clauses

Relative clauses in Crow are complex and subject to theoretical debates. There are two types of relative clauses in Crow: lexically headed and non-lexically headed. There are two basic relativizers /ak/ and /ala/, several composite forms based on ''ala'' plus ''baa'' 'indefinite pronoun' and instances with no relativizer. /ak/ indicates the subject of the relative clause is relativized and marks the subject as animate, and generally agenitive. It can occur in both lexically and non-lexically headed clauses. /ala/ may indicate a locative, temporal or manner adverbial is the head of the relative clause. In non-lexically headed relative clauses, /ala/ can sometimes be interpreted as the head of the clause itself. It can also occur in both lexically and non-lexically headed clauses. The relativizers are bound, with many exception, but they are generally prefixed to the word that contains the verb of the relative clause. Relative clauses are marked with final determiners. If the definite referent of the relative clause has already been accounted in the discourse or is otherwise obvious, the relative clause is marked with the definite /''-sh''/. Relative clauses can also be marked with the indefinite determiner marker /''-m''/; generally this is used to imply that the referent is being introduced into the discourse for the first time. However, the nominal head is almost always marked by the indefinite determiner /''-m''/.


References


Bibliography

*Graczyk, R. 2007. ''A Grammar of Crow: Apsáaloke Aliláau''. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. *Hollow, R.C., Jr.; Douglas R. Parks. Studies in Plains linguistics: a review. In ''Anthropology on the Great Plains'', ed. W. Raymond Wood and Margot Liberty. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. *Kaschube, D.V. 1978. ''Crow Texts''. IJAL-NATS Monograph No. 2. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. *Kaschube, D.V. 1963. ''Structural Elements of Crow''. Doctor of Philosophy Dissertation, Department of Anthropology, Indiana University, June 1960. Ann Arbor, MI: University Microfilms. *Kaschube, D.V. 1967. ''Structural Elements of Crow''. Boulder, CO: University of Colorado Press. *Lowie, R.H. 1945. The Crow Language: Grammatical Sketch and Analyzed Text. ''American Archaeology and Ethnology'', 39 (1942–1945):1-139. Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press. *Lowie, R.H. 1960. ''Crow Texts''. Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press. *Lowie, R.H. 1960. ''Crow Word Lists''. Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press. *Matthews, G.H. 1979. "Glottochronology and the separation of the Crow and Hidatsa." In ''Symposium on the Crow-Hidatsa Separations'', ed. by Leslie B. Davis. Bozeman, MT: Montana Archaeological Society. *Silver, S. & W.R. Miller. 1997. ''American Indian Languages: Cultural and Social Contexts''. Tucson: The University of Arizona Press.


External links

*Estes, J
How many indigenous American Languages Are Spoken in the United States? By how many speakers?
National Clearinghouse for Bilingual Education.

on the Native Languages of the Americas site

*
OLAC resources in and about the Crow language
{{DEFAULTSORT:Crow Language Crow tribe Endangered languages of the United States Indigenous languages of the North American Plains Native American language revitalization Western Siouan languages Indigenous languages of Montana Endangered indigenous languages of the Americas