Ambonese Malay
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Ambonese Malay or simply Ambonese is a Malay-based
creole language A creole language, or simply creole, is a stable natural language that develops from the simplifying and mixing of different languages into a new one within a fairly brief period of time: often, a pidgin evolved into a full-fledged language. ...
spoken on Ambon Island in the
Maluku Islands The Maluku Islands (; Indonesian: ''Kepulauan Maluku'') or the Moluccas () are an archipelago in the east of Indonesia. Tectonically they are located on the Halmahera Plate within the Molucca Sea Collision Zone. Geographically they are located ...
of
Eastern Indonesia This is a list of some of the regions of Indonesia. Many regions are defined in law or regulations by the central government. At different times of Indonesia's history, the nation has been designated as having regions that do not necessarily corr ...
. It was first brought by traders from Western Indonesia, then developed when the Dutch Empire
colonised Colonization, or colonisation, constitutes large-scale population movements wherein migrants maintain strong links with their, or their ancestors', former country – by such links, gain advantage over other inhabitants of the territory. When ...
the
Maluku Islands The Maluku Islands (; Indonesian: ''Kepulauan Maluku'') or the Moluccas () are an archipelago in the east of Indonesia. Tectonically they are located on the Halmahera Plate within the Molucca Sea Collision Zone. Geographically they are located ...
. This was the first example of the transliteration of Malay into the
Latin script The Latin script, also known as Roman script, is an alphabetic writing system based on the letters of the classical Latin alphabet, derived from a form of the Greek alphabet which was in use in the ancient Greek city of Cumae, in southern I ...
and it was used as a tool by missionaries in Eastern Indonesia. Malay has been taught in schools and churches in Ambon, and because of this it has become a lingua franca in Ambon and its surroundings. Christian speakers use Ambonese Malay as their mother tongue, while Muslims speak it as a second language as they have their own language. Muslims on Ambon Island particularly live in several areas in the city of
Ambon Ambon may refer to: Places * Ambon Island, an island in Indonesia ** Ambon, Maluku, a city on Ambon Island, the capital of Maluku province ** Governorate of Ambon, a colony of the Dutch East India Company from 1605 to 1796 * Ambon, Morbihan, a c ...
, dominant in the Salahutu and Leihitu Peninsulas. While in the Lease (/leɪ-a-seɪ/) Islands, the Christian Ambonese-speaking community is dominant in parts of Haruku, Saparua and Nusa Laut islands. Ambonese Malay has also become lingua franca in
Buru Buru (formerly spelled Boeroe, Boro, or Bouru) is the third largest island within the Maluku Islands of Indonesia. It lies between the Banda Sea to the south and Seram Sea to the north, west of Ambon and Seram islands. The island belongs to ...
,
Seram Seram (formerly spelled Ceram; also Seran or Serang) is the largest and main island of Maluku province of Indonesia, despite Ambon Island's historical importance. It is located just north of the smaller Ambon Island and a few other adjacent is ...
, Geser-Gorom and the south-western Maluku Islands, though with different accents. While originally derived from Malay, Ambonese Malay has been heavily influenced by European languages (Dutch and Portuguese) as well as the vocabularies or grammatical structures of indigenous languages. It is famous for its melodious accent. Muslims and Christian speakers tend to make different choices in vocabulary.
Papuan Malay Papuan Malay or Irian Malay is a Malay-based creole language spoken in the Indonesian part of New Guinea. It emerged as a contact language among tribes in Indonesian New Guinea (now Papua, Central Papua, Highland Papua, South Papua, and We ...
, a Malay creole spoken in the Indonesian part of
New Guinea New Guinea (; Hiri Motu: ''Niu Gini''; id, Papua, or , historically ) is the world's second-largest island with an area of . Located in Oceania in the southwestern Pacific Ocean, the island is separated from Australia by the wide Torr ...
, is closely related to Ambonese Malay and is said to be a derivative of Ambonese Malay or
Manado Malay Manado Malay, or simply the Manado language, is a creole language spoken in Manado, the capital of North Sulawesi province in Indonesia, and the surrounding area. The local name of the language is , and the name Minahasa Malay is also used, afte ...
or a mixture of both. According to Robert B. Allen and Rika Hayami-Allen, the eastern Indonesian forms of Malay have their roots in
North Moluccan Malay North Moluccan Malay (also known as Ternate Malay) is a Malay-based creole language spoken on Ternate, Tidore, Halmahera, and Sula Islands, North Maluku for intergroup communications. The local name of the language is ''Bahasa Pasar'', and the ...
.


Morpho-syntax


Pronouns and person markers

In Ambonese Malay, personal
pronoun In linguistics and grammar, a pronoun (abbreviated ) is a word or a group of words that one may substitute for a noun or noun phrase. Pronouns have traditionally been regarded as one of the parts of speech, but some modern theorists would not c ...
s typically have a full form, as well as another one or more shortened and/or variant forms.Van Minde 1997, p. 68
The pronouns vary in terms of
number A number is a mathematical object used to count, measure, and label. The original examples are the natural numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, and so forth. Numbers can be represented in language with number words. More universally, individual numbers c ...
- singular and plural, as well as
clusivity In linguistics, clusivity is a grammatical distinction between ''inclusive'' and ''exclusive'' first-person pronouns and verbal morphology, also called ''inclusive " we"'' and ''exclusive "we"''. Inclusive "we" specifically includes the addressee ...
, such as exclusive forms which exclude the addressee and inclusive forms which include the addressee. Such distinction is relatively typical of Austronesian languages. The following table provides a summary of all the pronouns found in Ambonese Malay:


Etymology

A number of observations can be made from the pronouns of Ambonese Malay which demonstrate etymology of certain pronouns: # A number of pronouns are historically compounded. They are: #* ''Bat'ong'': Derives from ''beta'' 'I (1SG) + ''orang'' 'people; man' #* ''Kat'ong'': Derives from *''kita'' 'we (1PL) + ''orang'' 'people; man' #* ''Dorang'': Derives from ''dia'' 'he; she; it (3SG) + ''orang'' 'people; man' # The 2nd person singular form ''ose'' is derived from the
Portuguese Portuguese may refer to: * anything of, from, or related to the country and nation of Portugal ** Portuguese cuisine, traditional foods ** Portuguese language, a Romance language *** Portuguese dialects, variants of the Portuguese language ** Portu ...
pronoun ''voce'', meaning 'you; thou'. # The 2nd person singular form ''ale'' is derived from a vernacular language.


Politeness

Similarly to other Austronesian languages, such as Malay and Indonesian, the 2nd person singular and one of the 3rd person singular pronouns in Ambonese Malay vary in their degree of politeness. They are summarised in the following table: It is also important to note that although in Ambonese the 1st person singular form ''beta'' is the standard form, in Classical Malay, it is used only by royal persons speaking to equals of rank.


= Syntactic positions

= As previously mentioned, Ambonese pronouns consist of a full and one or more variant form. Full forms occur in every syntactic position. Variant form have a more restricted distribution and may be functionally different. The following table summarises the set of full personal pronouns plus (in brackets) their variant forms according to context and syntactic function: From this table it follows that two factors determine whether a personal pronoun can be shortened: syntactic construction and syntactic position: * Except for the first person singular ''beta'', first person plural ''kat'ong'' and third person singular ''dia'', all pronouns can be shortened in one-word sentences. Third person singular neuter ''akang'' cannot form a one-word sentence altogether. * In clauses all personal pronouns in Subject position may be shortened, except for third person singular neutral ''akang''. * In clauses all personal pronouns in Object position may be shortened, except for first person singular ''beta'', first person plural ''kat'ong'', second person ''ale'' and third person singular ''dia''. * Likewise, as preposition object all personal pronouns except for ''beta'', ''ale'', ''kat'ong'' and ''dia'' may be shortened. These facts show that ''se'', ''os'' 'you', ''dong'' 'you', ''ont'o, ant'o, ant'u'' 'he; she' and ''dong'' 'they' have developed into doublets which are functionally (but not semantically) on a par with their full forms, while other short forms (''bet, al, kang, ang'') are phonological variants with a more restricted distribution. It is also important to note a number of syntactic variations within the functions of personal pronouns in Ambonese: 1) The 3rd person single ''dia'' 's/he; it' can be shortened to ''di'' or ''de'' when it is in Subject position, or when it is head of a Noun Phrase (NP) in object position. 2) The 3rd person single ''antua'' (and ''angtua, ontua, ongtua'') is also a modifier of head nominals in a phrase, thereby adding an aspect of deference. It adds a feature respect. 3) The third form, ''akang'', is a neater pronoun 'it', which also functions as a determiner. This form links up with the demonstratives ''ini'' and ''itu'' for deictic reference: it occurs as a single attribute before nouns, and in combination with postnominal ''tu''. 4) The short form of ''dorang'', which is ''dong'', also functions as a modifier in NPs to create collective plurals.


Reduplication

Reduplication with personal pronouns is not frequent. The following examples denote a concept "referent of pronoun plus persons who are alike":


Word Order

Ambonese Malay is a SVO (subject-verb-object) language. Its basic word order has the subject in initial position, followed by the verb and then the object, as shown below. 'Subject, verb, object' are labelled 'S, V, O' respectively:


Negation

Negation in Ambonese is sentential rather than constituent. Negation is predominantly expressed by five free morphemes that are treated as adverbs, modifying predicates, clauses or parts of the sentence as opposed to specific elements (such as single verbs or nominals). These morphemes are listed below alongside their common variants and English equivalents: * ''seng ‘no, not’'' * ''tar (tra) ‘no, not’'' * ''bal’ong (blong) ‘not yet’'' * ''bukang ‘no, not’'' * ''jangang (jang) ‘don’t’''.Van Minde 1997, p. 273 Constituent order Ambonese negators are typically positioned between the subject and the VO-group. This word order is typical of SVO languages. In the following example, the subject pronoun ''de'' precedes the negator ''seng (''represented here as ''sem''), and the verb group headed by the verb ''bisa'' follows it.


Summary of distinguishing features

# All negators except ''tar/tra'' can be used as one-word sentences in response to a Yes-No question.Van Minde 1997, p. 276 # All negators except ''jangang/jang'' can be used in declarative and interrogative sentence types. # Only ''jangang/jang'' can be used in an imperative sentence. These features will be explored in greater detail below.


''Seng''

''Seng'' is the most commonly used negator. Although Ambonese generally operates on the premise of sentential negation, ''seng'' can be used in a marked word order to narrow the scope of negation to single verbs or nominals. ''Seng'' moves rightwards in the clause, shifting the focus of negation to the word it immediately precedes. In the first example below, ''seng'' occurs in its default position between the subject ''de'' and the verb group headed by ''kar'ja,'' modifying the verb group ''kar'ja bat'ul'' in a general sense. In the second example, ''seng'' is integrated into the verb group itself, immediately preceding and placing the emphasis of negation on the verbal modifier ''bat'ul.'' ''Seng'' also collocates with modal auxiliary ''usa'' to express a lack of necessity: ''Seng'' is also used with the reduplicated interrogative pronoun ''apa,'' meaning 'what', in a fixed expression to denote 'nothing', or 'not anything':


''Tar/tra''

Unlike all other Ambonese negators, ''tar/tra'' cannot form a one-word sentence. It is typically regarded as a marker of emphatic negation, and can be used alongside ''seng'' and with reduplication to achieve even greater emphasis. In the example below, ''ampas'' is reduplicated, following both negators ''seng'' and ''tra.'' ''Tar/tra'' also commonly collocates with: * existential verb ''ada'' 'be (somewhere)' *''bai'' 'good' * modal auxiliary ''bole'' 'may, be allowed'


''Bal'ong/blong''

''Bal'ong'' marks both negation and the phasal aspect 'yet', denoting 'not yet'. Similar to ''tar/tra'' it can also be used as a marker of emphatic negation alongside a reduplicated verb, as in the example below where ''bal'ong'' co-occurs with the reduplicated verb ''pulang-pulang:''


''Bukang''

According to van Minde's research findings'', bukang'' is the least commonly used negator in Ambonese Malay.Van Minde 1997, p. 278 It expresses a 'contrast which implies an alternative' - ''bukang'' X means 'not X (but on the contrary) Y)': It may also occur in sentences with contrastive stress, combined with the use of 'higher pitch and articulatory strength' to articulate the constituents that are the focus of the negation. In the examples below, these specific constituents are capitalised to demonstrate prosodic emphasis:


''Jangang/jang''

''Jangang'' does not occur in declarative or interrogative sentences but is used to express negative imperatives, as in the example below - 'don't go to China.' It is also used in several 'short formulaic expressions':Van Minde 1997, p. 280


Negators and common collocations


= ''Lai''

= In the above example, the particle ''lai'' is used in sentence-final position as an exclamative marker. However, it can also be used with ''seng'', ''bal'ong,'' and ''jangang'' in non-exclamative sentences to denote, 'anymore; again', or 'still':


= ''Ka''

= ''Ka'' combines with negators ''seng'' and ''bal'ong'' to form a sentence-final tag for interrogative sentences: In the above example the tag ''ka seng'' has converted a yes-no question "Are you angry?" into an alternative question "Are you angry, or not?"


Phonology

Ambonese Malay has phonemic
word stress In linguistics, and particularly phonology, stress or accent is the relative emphasis or prominence given to a certain syllable in a word or to a certain word in a phrase or sentence. That emphasis is typically caused by such properties as i ...
, by which is meant that the position of stress within a word is unforeseeable (van Minde 1997, p. 21) . Van Minde (1997, p. 22) uses the term “lexically reduplicated morphemes” which means that both of the roots that compose the morpheme contain an important (e.g. stressed) syllable. However, in the case of duplicated
monosyllable In linguistics, a monosyllable is a word or utterance of only one syllable. It is most commonly studied in the fields of phonology and morphology and it has no semantic content. The word has originated from the Greek language. "Yes", "no", "jump", ...
s, neither of the roots are perceptible as regards stress. Each accent on the syllables will be marked even if the morpheme is made up of a duplicated monosyllable. The reason being is to differentiate them from morphemes that are monomorphemic (van Minde 1997, p. 22). Examples of this would be (p. 22): : /g’igi/ ‘tooth’; /pomp’om/ in: /s’agu pomp’om/ k.o sago-cake roasted in cartridge-cases. Compare the examples with the following (p. 23): : /’ eleK’ eleK/ ‘oil-lamp made from glass jar’; /t’omit’ omi/ k.o. cherry-like fruit. Wordstress is the only different feature in a number of
minimal pair In phonology, minimal pairs are pairs of words or phrases in a particular language, spoken or signed, that differ in only one phonological element, such as a phoneme, toneme or chroneme, and have distinct meanings. They are used to demonstrate ...
s (p. 23): : /b’acaŋ/ Bacan (island) - /bac’aŋ/ k.o. fruit : /p’araŋ/ ‘machete’ - /par’aŋ/ ‘war’ : /t’are/ ‘to attract s.t.’ - /tar’e/ ‘tight


Vowel phonemes

Ambonese contains 5 vowel phonemes as illustrated in the chart (van Minde 1997, p. 24): Ambonese Malay do not have phonemic glottal stop /ʔ/ but phonetically the glottal stop is noticed word-initially, morpheme-initially after a vowel, and morpheme-medially between like vowels (van Minde 1997, p. 24). Examples are (p. 24): : /’ose/ 'ʔ''’ ose‘you’ : /baku’atur/ aku#''ʔ’atur‘to arrange with one another’ In addition, there might be borrowed words from other indigenous languages, there is individual variation, and is occasionally heard (van Minde 1997, p. 24): : /pa’isal/ a''ʔ ‘''isal= a’isalk.o. small proa; : /n’oul. ’o''ʔ''ul= ’oulk.o. fish Nasalised vowels happen expectedly before nasal consonants belonging to the same syllable. Due to this, van Minde considers nasalization as a “phonetic phenomenon”. He gave some examples as well (1997, pp. 24–25): : /’aNbo''ŋ/ ’''ām.bõŋ‘Ambon; Ambonese’ : /kaNp’iNjaŋ/ ãm.p’ĩñ.jãŋ‘churchbell’ Nasalisation is invalid past syllable-boundaries like for example (p. 24-25): : /an’ioŋ/ 'ʔ''a.n’i.yõŋ‘rolled up cloth to support load carried on head’ : /ba#’aNbur/ a.'' ʔ''’āmbur‘to strew, scatter (repeatedly)


High front unrounded /i/

According to van Minde (1997, p. 25), the high front unrounded vowel /i/ is always perceived as and it always take place in non-final and final closed and open syllables. /i/ in final syllables (whether they are open or closed) can be replaced by /e/ however this only applies in some polysyllabic morphemes. Though /i/ can be replaced by /e/, it does not work in reverse thus it can’t be said the conflict between these two phonemes is negated in that position and environment (van Minde 1997, p. 25). Besides position and stress, further restrictions on the alternation /i ≈ e/ is given in two phonological rules (PR). The change in final unstressed syllables of polysyllabic morphemes is not attested in (van Minde, 1997 p. 25):
PR1: final syllables ending in /s/;
PR2: open final syllables when the penultimate syllable contains /u/ or /i/
Van Minde (1997, p. 25) regards /i/ as a ‘heavy phoneme’ wherever there is a change /i ~ e/, which means in environments that are different from those interpreted by PR1 and PR2. The definition of ‘heavy phoneme’ is defined as “consists of one or more optional distinctive features in addition to the basic distinctive features, whereas a basic phoneme consists of basic distinctive features only” (Ebeling 1967; Stokhof 1975). Van Minde deduces that /i/ is a heavy phoneme in environments excluded by PR1 and PR2; /e/ being its basic phoneme and elative highnessis a voluntary feature. Examples are given in the following (p. 25): : /katiNd’isaŋ/ atĩnd’isāŋ‘to talk or rave while sleep’ : /ir/ ir‘drunk’ * closed final syllables (p. 26): :: /k’ukis/ ’ukis *k’ukes‘biscuit’ :: /kac’il/ ac’il *kac’el‘small’ * open final syllables (p. 26): :: /p’uti/ ’uti *p’ute‘white’ :: /h’ari ≈ h’are/ ‘day’


Mid front unrounded vowel /e/

The mid front unrounded vowel /e/ is perceived as (or due to nasalization). Examples are followed (p. 26): : /b’esi/ ’esi‘iron/steel’ : /b’erkaT/ ’erkat-‘divine blessing’ The phonemic status of /e/ versus /i/ is attested by the followed minimal pairs (p. 26): : /’ina/ ‘mother’ - /’ena/ ‘tasty, delicious’ : /p’ici/ ‘to peel’ - /p’eci/ ‘mud’ The examples illustrated distinctly that /i/ is resistant to /e/ in morpheme-final syllables, hence the change /i ≈ e/ in final syllables under the previous restrictions stated in the phonological rules cannot be clarified as neutralization (van Minde 1997, pp. 26–27).


Low central vowel /a/

The low central vowel in Ambonese Malay is perceived as (or due to nasalization). Examples are followed (p. 27): : /’añer/ ’añer‘bad fishy smell’ : /kal’apa/ al’apa‘coconut’ The minimal pairs attest the resistance between /a/ and /i/ (p. 27): : /s’atu/ ‘one’ - /s’itu/ ‘there’ : /bal’a/ ‘to split s.t.’ - /bal’i/ ‘to buy s.t.’ The opposition between /a/ and /e/ is shown by the presented minimal pairs (p. 27): : /cak’a/ ‘to strangle s.o.’ - /cak’e/ ‘(vulg) to eat, stuff o.s.’ : /b’aca/ ‘to read’ - /b’eca/ ‘pedicab’


High back rounded vowel /u/

/u/ happens in non-final and final closed and open position. This high back rounded vowel is always perceived as (or due to nasalization). However, /u/ in final unstressed syllables (whether open or closed) consistently alternates with /o/ when in certain polysyllabic morphemes (van Minde 1997, p. 27). In addition, not all /o/ in this position and environment alternates with /u/, thus these two phonemes are not balanced. According to van Minde (1997, p. 27), the change /u ≈ o/ in final unstressed syllables of polysyllabic morphemes is not proven in (p. 27):
PR3: open final syllables when the penultimate syllable have /u/ or /i/
This situation is alike to the change between the high and mid front vowels /i ≈ e/, where /u/ is seen as a heavy phoneme, /o/ as the basic phoneme and elative phonemeis the optional feature. The following examples prove this assertion (p. 28): : /t’obu/ ’obu≈ /t’obo/ ’obo‘sugar cane’ : /’uNpaŋ/ ’ ũmpãŋ‘bait’ * Closed final syllables (p. 28): :: /c’aNpur ≈ c’aNpor/ ‘to mix’ :: /c’abuT ≈ c’aboT/ ‘let’s go!’ * Open final syllables (p. 29): :: /tapar’egu ≈ tapar’ego/ ‘to do s.t. hurriedly’ :: /cap’atu ≈ cap’ato/ ‘shoe’ * Minimal pairs shown to the resistance between /u/ and /i/ (p. 29): :: /k’utu/ ‘louse’ - /k’uti/ ‘snap’ :: /g’ula/ ‘sugar’ - /g’ila/ ‘mad’ * The resistance between /u/ and /e/ can be shown by (p. 29): :: /b’ulaŋ/ ‘moon, month’- /b’elaŋ/ in: /aruNb’ aI b’elaŋ/ k.o. large proa The resistance between /u/ and /a/ is attested by: : /’uru/ ‘to massage’ – /’aru/ ‘Aru Islands’


Mid back rounded vowel /o/

The mid back rounded vowel /o/ is seen as (or due to nasalization). Examples are followed (p. 29): : /’oraŋ/ orãŋ‘man, human being’ : /b’odo/ ’odo‘stupid’ * The minimal pair show the phoneme status of /o/ vis-à-vis /i/ (p. 30): :: /b’oŋko/ ‘bowed (of persons)’ – /b’eŋko/ ‘bend sideways (of persons)’ * The phoneme status of /o/ against /a/ is shown by (p. 30): :: /kat’oŋ/ ‘we’ - /kat’ aŋ/ ‘crab’ * The resistance between /o/ and /u/ is proven by (p. 30): :: /h’ori/ ‘to wander about’ – /h’uri/ ‘having lost its original shape’


Archiphoneme /U/

Archiphoneme In phonology and linguistics, a phoneme () is a unit of sound that can distinguish one word from another in a particular language. For example, in most dialects of English, with the notable exception of the West Midlands and the north-w ...
/U/ is proposed in unstressed position after a vowel other than /u/ and instantly before a syllable or morpheme boundary by van Minde (1997). This is because there is no opposition between /u/ and /w/ in this environment (p. 31): : /l’aUtaŋ/ ’autãŋ‘deep-sea’ The second reason proposed by van Minde (1997, p. 32) is that /U/ occurs after a consonant and directly before a stressed vowel (p. 32): : /bU’aya/ u’aya‘crocodile’ Nevertheless, not every pattern /Cw’V/ is collateral by a sequence /CU’V/ (p. 32): : /swaK/ wak-‘weak, feeble’


Archiphoneme /I/

This results from the neutralization of the resistance /i/-/y/ in unstressed position after a vowel or instantly before a stressed vowel (p. 32): : /m’uI/ ’ui, muy‘aunt’ * Van Minde (1997, p. 32) showed some examples of /I/ before a stressed vowel: :: /I’a/ ’a, iy’a‘yes (formal)’ :: /bI’asa/ i’asa‘normal, usual’ * The resistance of /i/ and /y/ is shown by (p. 33): :: /t’iU/ ’iyu‘uncle’


Consonants

There are 19
consonant In articulatory phonetics, a consonant is a speech sound that is articulated with complete or partial closure of the vocal tract. Examples are and pronounced with the lips; and pronounced with the front of the tongue; and pronounced wi ...
s phonemes and 4 consonant
archiphoneme In phonology and linguistics, a phoneme () is a unit of sound that can distinguish one word from another in a particular language. For example, in most dialects of English, with the notable exception of the West Midlands and the north-w ...
s in Ambonese Malay and they are charted below (van Minde 1997, pp. 40–41):


Archiphonemes /P, T, K, N/

These archiphonemes is a consequence from the neutralization of the opposition between /p/- /b/, /t/-/d/, and /k/-/g/ respectively in two positions and under certain conditions (van Minde 1997, p. 40): 1. Frequency of /P, T, K/ is especially high in word-final position. Most words with /P, T, K/ in this position are borrowed words from Dutch (van Minde 1997, p. 41). This is due to the many words of Malay origin to have lost final stops *p, *t, *k, or *ʔ. Example: ''*tutup > tutu ‘to close’.'' Wordfinally the archiphonemes /P, T, K/ have a voiceless unreleased realization. Examples are shown (p. 41): : /keP/ ep-‘notch, nick’ (’ãmbak-‘character, habit’ A significant number of polysyllabic words with non-final stress have a doublet without final /P, T, K/ in non emphatic speech (p. 41): : /r’unuT ≈ runu/ ‘fibroid material growing on palm trees’ In addition, van Minde (1997, p. 42) states that /P, T, K/ are “heavy archiphonemes” in this position and environment. 2. The archiphonemes /P, T, K/ have a voiceless unreleased realization in syllable-final position right before a stop, a nasal, fricative /s/, or the lateral /l/. Example is shown (p. 42): : /b’aKso/ ’ak-so‘(Chinese) meatball’ Archiphoneme /N/ is also developed from the neutralization of the opposition between /m, n, ñ/ and /ŋ/ before their own homorgonaic obstruents /l/ (van Minde 1997, p. 42). Example is followed (p. 42): : /naNl’ohi/ ãnl’ohiNanlohi (clan name)


Stops

The voiceless and voiced stops of the series /p, b, t, d, c, j, k, g/ are found in word-medial position and word-initial. /p, b/ are bilabials, /d/ is an apico-alveolar, /c, j/ are laminal-palatals, /t/ is an apico-alveodental, /k, g/ are dorsal-velars. Relevant (near) minimal pairs are shown below (van Minde 1997, pp. 43–44): * ''p: b /t’aNpa/ ‘place stop’; /t’aNba/ ‘to add s.t.’'' * ''p: t /p’aku/ ‘nail’; /t’aku/ ‘afraid’'' * ''p:d /b’apa/ ‘term of address’; /bad’a/ ‘skin powder’'' * ''p:c /p’ipi/ ‘cheek’; /p’ici/ ‘to give money’'' * ''p:j /p’ari/ ‘ray (fish)’; /j’ari/ ‘finger’'' * ''p:k /p’ele/ ‘to obstruct’; /k’ele/ ‘to press under arm’'' * ''p:g /p’araŋ/ ‘machete’; /g’araŋ/ ‘salt’'' * ''b:t /b’aru/ ‘new’; /t’aru/ ‘to place’'' * ''b:d /b’apa/ ‘term of address’; /d’apa/ ‘to obtain’'' * ''b:c /b’ole/ ‘may’; /c’ole/ ‘bodice’'' * ''b:j /b’aNbaŋ/ ‘to warn’; /j’aNbaŋ/ ‘beard’'' * ''b:k /b’ore/ ‘poisonous extract’; /k’ore/ ‘to fumble’'' * ''b:g /b’aru/ ‘new’; /g’aru/ ‘to scratch’'' * ''t:d /t’ulaŋ/ ‘bone’; /d’ulaŋ/ ‘wooden tray’'' * ''t:c /t’ari/ ‘dance’; /c’ari/ ‘to search’'' * ''t:j /t’ari/ ‘dance’; /j’ari/ ‘finger’'' * ''t:k /t’anaŋ/ ‘to plant’; /k’anaŋ/ ‘right (side)’'' * ''t:g /t’ali/ ‘rope’; /g’ali/ ‘to dig’'' * ''d:c /d’aro/ ‘from’; /c’ari/ ‘to search’'' * ''d:j /d’aro/ ‘from’; /j’ari/ ‘finger’'' * ''d:k /d’aki/ ‘dirt’; /k’aki/ ‘foot’'' * ''d:g /d’oti/ ‘to work magic’; /g’oti/ ‘trough for preparing sago’'' * ''c:g /c’ari/ ‘search’; /j’ari/ ‘finger'' * ''c:k /coK/ ‘choke’; /koK/ ‘owl’'' * ''c:g /c’ili/ ‘chili’; /g’ilig’ili/ ‘tickle’'' * ''j:k /j’aNji/ ‘to agree’; /k’aNji/ ‘starch’'' * ''j:g /j’aga/ ‘to keep watch’; /g’aga/ ‘beautiful’'' * ''k:g /k’araŋ/ ‘coral’; /g’araŋ/ ‘salt’'' In certain lexical items there is an unexplained change between the voiceless stop and its homorganic voiced counterpart (van Minde 1997, p. 44): : /cigulu/ = /jigulu/ ‘riddle’


Nasals

The nasals /m, n, ñ, ŋ/ are separated on the basis of the following (near-) minimal pairs (van Minde 1997, pp. 44–45): * ''m:n /m’ani/ ‘bead’; /n’ani/ ‘bamboo hoe’'' * ''m:ñ /m’adu/ ‘honey’; /ñ’adu/ ‘brother/sister in-law’'' * ''m:ŋ /m’ana/ ‘where’; /ŋ’ana/ ‘(inter.) gee!’'' * ''n:ñ /ne/ in /b’ubur ne/ k.o. porridge; /ñe/ in” /m’uka m’acaŋ ñe/ ‘look disappointed’'' * ''m:ŋ /’ina/ ‘mother’; /’iŋa/ ‘to remember’'' * ''ñ:ŋ /t’aña/ ‘to ask’; /t’eŋa/ ‘middle’'' The prevalence and functional load of these nasal contrasts differently. They take place word-initially and word-medially before vowels. However, in the environment of word-initially, the functional load of /ñ/ and /ŋ/ is low. Examples are shown (van Minde 1997, p. 45): : /ñ’amu/ ’amu‘mosquito’ : /ñoŋ/ õŋ‘term of address for boys’ In morpheme-final position, nasals other than /ŋ/ are barely proven and thus van Minde (1997, p. 46) finds it hard to find minimal pairs that are different in morpheme-final nasal phoneme.


Fricatives

The labio-dental
fricative A fricative is a consonant produced by forcing air through a narrow channel made by placing two articulators close together. These may be the lower lip against the upper teeth, in the case of ; the back of the tongue against the soft palate in t ...
/f/ takes place only in borrowed words and in words of unknown origin. Examples are shown (van Minde 1997, p. 46): : /f’ader/ ’ader‘term of address for men’ The alveolar fricative /s/ happens in word-initial, -medial, and –final position. Examples are shown (p. 47): : /saw’aI/ aw’ai‘awry’ : /rab’us/ ab’us‘to boil’ The glottal fricative /h/ takes place word-medially and word-initially. However, in the use of interjections such as /ih/ ‘Hey!’ and /ah/ ‘Oh no!’- /h/ occur word-finally (van Minde 1997, p. 47) . Also, word-medial /h/ is optional in certain words; when /h/ is removed between like vowels, one of the two adjacent vowel segments is also deleted. Examples are illustrated below (p. 47): : /bah’asa/ ≈ /basa/ ‘language’ : /mas’ohi/ ≈ /mas’oI/ ‘mutual aid’


Liquids

The liquids /l/ and /r/ take place in word-initial, -medial, -final position, just like the alveolar fricative /s/. Example is shown (van Minde 1997, p. 48): : /t’ari/ ’ari‘dance’


Semivowels

The
semivowel In phonetics and phonology, a semivowel, glide or semiconsonant is a sound that is phonetically similar to a vowel sound but functions as the syllable boundary, rather than as the nucleus of a syllable. Examples of semivowels in English are the c ...
s /w/ and /y/ happen in word-initial and word-medial position before a vowel. Example are shown below (van Minde 1997, p. 48): : /hay’al/ ay’al‘to flirt’ : /kal’uyu/ al’uyu‘shark’


Samples

Examples: * ''Beta pung nama Ahmad'' = My name is Ahmed * ''Ose su tau Ahmad pung maitua?'' = Do you know Ahmed's wife? * ''Jang bakudapa deng dia dolo, dia ada gagartang deng ose'' = Don't meet with him for a moment, he's angry with you. * ''Susi dong pung kaka mo pi kamari'' = Susi's brother will come * ''Ini beta kasi akang voor ose'' = This is for you. * ''Ale badiang jua, beta cumang mo tipu-tipu. Tuang Ala'' = Shut up, I am just tricking them. god! ('god!' as in swearing. eg. 'jesus christ!) * ''Beta seng tau'' = I don't know Ambonese word samples: * ''Beta'' = I * ''Ose, Ale'' = you (ose is derived from the Portuguese ''voce'') * ''Dia'' = he, she * ''Akang'' = (may) it * ''Katong'' = we (cut from ''kita orang'') * ''Dong'' = they (cut from ''dia orang'') * ''Kamong'', ''kamorang'' = you (pl) (cut from ''kamu orang'') * ''Antua'' = he, she (respectful form) * ''iyo'' = yes * ''seng'' = no * ''bakubae'' = peace * ''nanaku'' = pay attention to something * ''su'' = already (indicating something has already happened or has been done)
INTER:interjection PHA:phasal aspect marker


See also

*
North Moluccan Malay North Moluccan Malay (also known as Ternate Malay) is a Malay-based creole language spoken on Ternate, Tidore, Halmahera, and Sula Islands, North Maluku for intergroup communications. The local name of the language is ''Bahasa Pasar'', and the ...
*
Papuan Malay Papuan Malay or Irian Malay is a Malay-based creole language spoken in the Indonesian part of New Guinea. It emerged as a contact language among tribes in Indonesian New Guinea (now Papua, Central Papua, Highland Papua, South Papua, and We ...
* Serui Malay


Notes


References

* Ethnologue. (2015). Malay, Ambonese. Retrieved 8 May 2015, from https://www.ethnologue.com/language/abs * Nordhoff, Sebastian; Hammarström, Harald; Forkel, Robert; Haspelmath, Martin, eds. (2013). "Ambonese Malay". Glottolog. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. * Van Minde, D. (1997). Malayu Ambong. Leiden, the Netherlands: Research School CNWS.


External links

* Materials on Ambonese Malay are included in the open access
Arthur Capell Arthur Capell (28 March 1902 – 10 August 1986) was an Australian linguist, who made major contributions to the study of Australian languages, Austronesian languages and Papuan languages. Early life Capell was born in Newtown, New South Wales ...
collection
AC1
held by Paradisec. {{authority control Agglutinative languages Languages of Indonesia Malay-based pidgins and creoles