HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

In certain theories of
linguistics Linguistics is the science, scientific study of human language. It is called a scientific study because it entails a comprehensive, systematic, objective, and precise analysis of all aspects of language, particularly its nature and structure ...
, thematic relations, also known as semantic roles, are the various roles that a
noun phrase In linguistics, a noun phrase, or nominal (phrase), is a phrase that has a noun or pronoun as its head or performs the same grammatical function as a noun. Noun phrases are very common cross-linguistically, and they may be the most frequently oc ...
may play with respect to the action or state described by a governing verb, commonly the sentence's main verb. For example, in the sentence "Susan ate an apple", ''Susan'' is the doer of the eating, so she is an agent; ''an apple'' is the item that is eaten, so it is a
patient A patient is any recipient of health care services that are performed by healthcare professionals. The patient is most often ill or injured and in need of treatment by a physician, nurse, optometrist, dentist, veterinarian, or other hea ...
. Since their introduction in the mid 1960s by Jeffrey Gruber and Charles Fillmore, semantic roles have been a core linguistic concept and ground of debate between linguist approaches, because of their potential in explaining the relationship between syntax and semantics (also known as the syntax-semantics interface), that is how meaning affects the surface syntactic codification of language. The notion of semantic roles play a central role especially in functionalist and language-comparative ( typological) theories of language and grammar. While most modern linguistic theories make reference to such relations in one form or another, the general term, as well as the terms for specific relations, varies: "participant role", "semantic role", and "deep case" have also been employed with similar sense.


History

The notion of semantic roles was introduced into theoretical linguistics in the 1960s, by Jeffrey Gruber and Charles Fillmore,Van Valin Jr, R. D. (2008).
A6 Frame Semantics for Verbs
'. Functional Concepts and Frames – Proposal.
Gruber (1965), Fillmore (1968) and also Jackendoff did some early work on it in 1972. The focus of these studies on semantic aspects, and how they affect syntax, was part of a shift away from Chomsky's syntactic-centered approach, and in particular the notion of the
autonomy of syntax In linguistics, the autonomy of syntax is the assumption that syntax is arbitrary and self-contained with respect to meaning, semantics, pragmatics, discourse function, and other factors external to language.Croft (1995) Autonomy and Functionalis ...
, and his recent '' Aspects of the Theory of Syntax'' (1965).


Major thematic relations

The following major thematic relations have been identified:


Agent

: deliberately performs the action (e.g. ''Bill'' ate his soup quietly). The actions can be both conscious or unconscious. In syntax, the agent is the argument of a transitive verb that corresponds to the subject in English.


Experiencer

: the entity that receives sensory or emotional input (e.g. ''Susan'' heard the song. ''I'' cried).


Stimulus

: entity that prompts sensory or emotional feeling – not deliberately (e.g. David loves ''onions!'').


Theme

: undergoes the action but does not change its state (e.g. We believe in one ''God''. I have ''two children''. I put ''the book'' on the table. He gave ''the gun'' to the police officer.) (Sometimes used interchangeably with patient.) In syntax, the theme is the direct object of a ditransitive verb.


Patient A patient is any recipient of health care services that are performed by healthcare professionals. The patient is most often ill or injured and in need of treatment by a physician, nurse, optometrist, dentist, veterinarian, or other hea ...

: undergoes the action and changes its state (e.g. The falling rocks crushed ''the car''.). (Sometimes used interchangeably with theme.) In syntax, the patient is the single object of a (mono)transitive verb.


Instrument

: used to carry out the action (e.g. Jamie cut the ribbon ''with a pair of scissors''.).


Force or natural cause

: mindlessly performs the action (e.g. ''An avalanche'' destroyed the ancient temple.).


Location

: where the action occurs (e.g. Johnny and Linda played carelessly ''in the park''. I'll be ''at Julie's house'' studying for my test.).


Direction or goal

: where the action is directed towards (e.g. The caravan continued on ''toward the distant oasis''. He walked ''to school''.).


Recipient

: a special kind of goal associated with verbs expressing a change in ownership, possession (e.g. I sent ''John'' the letter. He gave the book ''to her''). In syntax, the recipient or goal is the indirect object of a ditransitive verb.


Source or origin

: where the action originated (e.g. The rocket was launched ''from Central Command''. She walked ''away from him''.).


Time

: the time at which the action occurs (e.g. The pitcher struck out nine batters ''today'')


Beneficiary or recipient

: the entity for whose benefit the action occurs (e.g. I baked ''Reggie'' a cake. He built a car ''for me''. I fight ''for the king''.).


Manner

: the way in which an action is carried out (e.g. ''With great urgency'', Tabitha phoned 911.).


Purpose

: the reason for which an action is performed (e.g. Tabitha phoned 911 right away ''in order to get some help''.).


Cause

: what caused the action to occur in the first place; not ''for what'', rather ''because of what'' (e.g. ''Because Clyde was hungry'', he ate the cake.). There are not always clear boundaries between these relations. For example, in "the hammer broke the window", ''hammer'' might be labeled an agent (see below), an instrument, a force, or possibly a cause. Nevertheless, some thematic relation labels are more logically plausible than others.


Grouping into the two macroroles of actor and undergoer

In many functionally oriented linguistic approaches, the above thematic roles have been grouped into the two macroroles (also called generalized semantic roles or proto-roles) of ''actor'' and ''undergoer''. This notion of semantic macroroles was introduced by Van Valin's Ph.D. thesis in 1977, developed in
role and reference grammar Role and reference grammar (RRG) is a model of grammar developed by William A. Foley and Robert Van Valin, Jr. in the 1980s, which incorporates many of the points of view of current functional grammar theories. In RRG, the description of a sent ...
, and then adapted in several linguistic approaches.Van Valin, R. D. (1999).
Generalized semantic roles and the syntax-semantics interface
In: ''Empirical issues in formal syntax and semantics'', 2, 373–389.
According to Van Valin, while thematic roles define semantic relations, and relations like subject and direct object are syntactic ones, the semantic macroroles of actor and undergoer are relations that lie at the interface between semantics and syntax.Van Valin Jr, R. D. (2005).
Exploring the syntax-semantics interface
', p. 67. Cambridge University Press.
Linguistic approaches that have adopted, in various forms, this notion of semantic macroroles include: the Generalized Semantic Roles of Foley and Van Valin Role and reference grammar (1984), David Dowty’s 1991 theory of thematic proto-roles,Dowty D.R (1991). Thematic Proto-Roles and Argument Selection. Language 67: 547-619 Kibrik's Semantic hyperroles (1997),
Simon Dik Simon Cornelis Dik (September 6, 1940 in Delden – March 1, 1995 in Holysloot) was a Dutch linguist, most famous for developing the theory of functional grammar. He occupied the chair of General Linguistics at University of Amsterdam betwe ...
's 1989
Functional discourse grammar Functional grammar (FG) and functional discourse grammar (FDG) are grammar models and theories motivated by functional theories of grammar. These theories explain how linguistic utterances are shaped, based on the goals and knowledge of natural lan ...
, and some late 1990s versions of Head-driven phrase structure grammar.Bornkessel, I., Schlesewsky, M., Comrie, B. & Friederici, A. (2009).
Introduction - Semantic Roles as a core linguistic concept
', pp.1-2, in I. Bornkessel et al. (Eds), ''Semantic Role Universals and Argument Linking'' (pp. 1-14). Berlin, New York: De Gruyter Mouton.
Van Valin Jr, R. D. (2004)
Semantic macroroles in role and reference grammar
' p.62-4, in ''Semantische rollen'', pp. 62-82.
In Dowty’s theory of thematic proto-roles, semantic roles are considered as prototype notions, in which there is a prototypical agent role that has those traits characteristically associated to it, while other thematic roles have less of those traits and are accordingly proportionally more distant to the prototypical agent.Rappaport Hovav, M., & Levin, B. (2015).
The Syntax‐Semantics Interface
', pp. 602–603 in The handbook of contemporary semantic theory, pp. 593-624.
The same goes for the opposite pole of the continuum, the patient proto-role.


Relationship to case

In many languages, such as
Finnish Finnish may refer to: * Something or someone from, or related to Finland * Culture of Finland * Finnish people or Finns, the primary ethnic group in Finland * Finnish language, the national language of the Finnish people * Finnish cuisine See also ...
, Hungarian and Turkish, thematic relations may be reflected in the case-marking on the noun. For instance, Hungarian has an instrumental case ending (-val/-vel), which explicitly marks the instrument of a sentence. Languages like English often mark such thematic relations with prepositions.


Conflicting terminologies

The term ''thematic relation'' is frequently confused with theta role. Many linguists (particularly
generative grammar Generative grammar, or generativism , is a linguistic theory that regards linguistics as the study of a hypothesised innate grammatical structure. It is a biological or biologistic modification of earlier structuralist theories of linguisti ...
ians) use the terms interchangeably. This is because theta roles are typically named by the most prominent thematic relation that they are associated with. To make matters more confusing, depending upon which theoretical approach one assumes, the grammatical relations of subject and object, etc., are often closely tied to the semantic relations. In the typological tradition, for example, agents/actors (or "agent-like" arguments) frequently overlap with the notion of subject (S). These ideas, when they are used distinctly, can be distinguished as follows: ; Thematic relations : are purely semantic descriptions of the way in which the entities described by the noun phrase are functioning with respect to the meaning of the action described by the verb. A noun may bear more than one thematic relation. Almost every noun phrase bears at least one thematic relation (the exception are expletives). Thematic relations on a noun are identical in sentences that are paraphrases of one another. ; Theta roles : are syntactic structures reflecting positions in the argument structure of the verb they are associated with. A noun may only bear one theta role. Only arguments bear theta roles. Adjuncts do not bear theta roles. ; Grammatical relations : express the surface position (in languages like English) or case (in languages like Latin) that a noun phrase bears in the sentence. Thematic relations concern the nature of the relationship between the meaning of the verb and the meaning of the noun. Theta roles are about the number of arguments that a verb requires (which is a purely syntactic notion). Theta roles are syntactic relations that refers to the semantic thematic relations. For example, take the sentence "Reggie gave the kibble to Fergus on Friday." * Thematic relations: ''Reggie'' is doing the action so is the agent, but he is also the source of the kibble (note Reggie bears two thematic relations); ''the kibble'' is the entity acted upon so it is the patient; Fergus is the direction/goal or recipient of the giving. Friday represents the time of the action. * theta roles: The verb ''give'' requires three arguments (see valency). In generative grammar, this is encoded in terms of the number and type of theta roles the verb takes. The theta role is named by the most prominent thematic relation associated with it. So the three required arguments bear the theta roles named the agent (Reggie) the patient (or theme) (the kibble), and goal/recipient (Fergus). ''On Friday'' does not receive a theta role from the verb, because it is an adjunct. Note that ''Reggie'' bears two thematic relations (Agent and Source), but only one theta role (the argument slot associated with these thematic relations). * grammatical relations: The subject (S) of this sentence is ''Reggie'', the object (O) is ''the kibble'', the indirect object is ''to Fergus'', and ''on Friday'' is an oblique.


See also

*
Morphosyntactic alignment In linguistics, morphosyntactic alignment is the grammatical relationship between arguments—specifically, between the two arguments (in English, subject and object) of transitive verbs like ''the dog chased the cat'', and the single argument ...
* Case grammar * Theta roles *
Semantic role labeling In natural language processing, semantic role labeling (also called shallow semantic parsing or slot-filling) is the process that assigns labels to words or phrases in a sentence that indicates their semantic role in the sentence, such as that of ...
, a natural language processing task to automatically determine thematic roles * Lexical function


Inline references


Further references

* Carnie, Andrew. 2007.
Syntax: A Generative introduction
'. 2nd Edition. Blackwell Publishers. * Davis, Anthony R.: ''Thematic roles''. In: Claudia Maienborn, Klaus von Heusinger, Paul Portner (Hrsg.): ''Semantics: an international handbook of natural language meaning''. Vol. 1. Berlin 2011, S. 399–420.handbook of natural language meaning. Vol. 1. Berlin 2011, S. 399–420. * * Fillmore, Charles. 1968. The Case for Case. In Universals in Linguistic Theory, eds. Emmon Bach and R.T. Harms. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. * Fillmore, Charles. 1971
Types of lexical information
In Semantics. An interdisciplinary reader in philosophy, linguistics and psychology, eds. D. Steinberg and L. Jacobovitz: Cambridge University Press. * (Chapter V. Thematic Roles, pp. 197–249) * Angela D. Friederici, Anja Hahne, Axel Mecklinger: ''Temporal structure of syntactic parsing. Early and late event-related potential effects.'' In: ''Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition.'' 22-5, (1996), S. 1219–1248. * Gruber, Jeffrey. 1965. Studies in lexical relations, MIT: Ph.D. * Gruber, Jeffrey ''Thematic relations in syntax.'' In: Mark R. Baltin, Chris Collins (Hrsg.): ''The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory.'' Blackwell, Oxford 2000, ISBN 0-631-20507-1, S. 257–298. * Harley, Heidi. In press. Thematic Roles. In Patrick Hogan, ed. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Cambridge University Press. * Higginbotham, James (1999) ''Thematic Roles'', pp. 837-8, in:
The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences
', Edited by Keil & Wilson (1999) Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. * Jackendoff, Ray. 1983.
Semantics and cognition
'. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. * Jackendoff, Ray. 1990.
Semantic structures
'. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. * McRae, Ken and Ferretti, Todd R. and Amyote, Liane: ''Thematic roles as verb-specific concepts.'' In: ''Language and cognitive processes.'' 12-2/3, (1997) 137–176. * Primus, Beatrice: ''Semantische Rollen.'' Winter, Heidelberg 2012, ISBN 978-3-8253-5977-5 * Primus, Beatrice: ''Participant roles''. In: Nick Riemer (Hrsg.): ''The Routledge Handbook of Semantics''. London 2016, S. 403–418. * Van Valin, Robert (2008) ''Introduction to Syntax.'' Cambridge University Press, Cambridge 2008, ISBN 0-521-63566-7 * Van Valin Jr, R. D. (1977).
Aspects of Lakhota Syntax
'. University of California, Berkeley. {{Authority control Thematic roles Semantics